This is the old United Nations University website. Visit the new site at http://unu.edu
In the previous section, I have dealt with the factors affecting the desirability and intake of particular foods from sensory and cultural symbolic perspectives, and have mentioned only in passing the dietary structures into which such foods are integrated. This section will deal briefly with the two major dimensions of dietary construction meal patterns and rules for food sharing and the way anthropologists study them.
Dietary Structure
Beyond individual factors in food selection, people also structure consumption in terms of dietary patterns. These include ordinary daily rounds of meals and snacks, as well as annual cycles of feasts and fast days, which in combination with classifications of individual foods comprise the group's "food habits." How often one eats, the times of day or night that one eats, the kinds of food one eats in general and on each eating occasion, and with whom one eats are ways of communicating information about one's own social and cultural identity and relationships with others. The "food code" - the kinds of foods (for instance, cooked versus uncooked) one consumes at particular times of the day - and the individuals one invites to share each repast are basic to deciphering the cultural rules of etiquette, identity, and membership in social groups (Douglas, 1972). They provide an additional key to criteria for food selection and individual nutrition.
Food patterns are constructed by encoding foods according to their proper place in the diet. Foods may be classified as "meal items" including staples such as cereal grains, tubers, and plantains; "relishes" (i.e. preparations eaten along with staples) and "condiments" such as salt, mineral earths, and chili peppers; or as categories appropriate to particular meals, such as "toast" for breakfast and "meat" and "potatoes" for dinner foods in American society (Lewin, 1943). Alternatively, they may be deemed "snack foods," i.e. those that are "uncooked" and consumed without the major cooked staple or eaten outside of the regular meal settings. Or, certain dishes may be considered as special foods eaten only at feasts or as famine food consumed only if other more preferred foods are unavailable.
Studies in the "foodways" of different groups classify foods into "core," "secondary core" ("specialty") and "peripheral" ("alternative") items in order to describe the staple versus more variable elements of cultural diets (Passin and Bennett, 1943). Meal planning may also be studied according to folk categories such as the Italian-American mode of varying in meals the basic elements of "pasta" and "gravy" (Theophano, 1982; Curtis, 1983).
What constitutes a "meal," as opposed to a "snack," is defined in every culture, and this definition determines what foods are appropriate at different times of the day. In Malaysia, for example, cooked rice defines a "meal"; all food eaten without cooked rice is defined as a "snack." For those recording dietary intake, it is useful to realize that "snack" foods may not be listed in recall surveys because they may not be considered "real foods." Also, foods considered inferior and used only in times of scarcity or those consumed ordinarily only by infants or foraging youngsters may go unreported even if they are part of dietary intake of the young or of adults.
How many meals are prepared and their timing during a day is also culturally determined. The number, timing, structure, and contents of meals may be relatively fixed, as in upper-class British society (Douglas, 1972), or the number of meals and ingredients may vary throughout the year according to seasonal resources and festivities. Many African horticultural societies adjust downward from two meals to one meal a day and rely on more or less palatable wild foods as harvest stores are depleted. Rationing for the purpose of extending limited resources is common in most cultures without adequate surplus food supplies, although the timing of such rationing and the mechanisms by which people compute how much food to allow for daily meals has not been carefully studied.
If firewood or other fuel supplies are short and/or expensive, and if the food provider-preparer has other work responsibilities, the number of meals defined as "cooked food" may be restricted. Employment or school schedules also affect how work or meals structure the day in modern industrialized societies. Rotenberg (1981) analysed how changing work schedules in post-Second World War Viennese society caused a shift from five to three meals a day as well as the increased consumption of convenience (prepared) foods.
Cultural group membership may also be marked by the festival periods of feasting and fasting that celebrate group cohesion. Even ordinary food patterns, such as what constitutes appropriate meal and snack foods, may mark cultural (ethnic) group membership, for example in the case of American Poles and Blacks (Kolasa, 1978; Jerome, 1979). Factors of income levels, perceived prestige values, and availability of fresh foods and other ingredients in different cultural contexts also affect continuities in ethnic dietary structure, as does the ongoing presence of a social group that shares such ethnic foodways.
In addition to examining multiple socio-cultural determinants of food selection and dietary construction, nutritionists have tried to quantify qualitative aspects of dietary patterns and relate them to nutritional content and adequacy of diets. "Food diversity indices" or "dietary complexity" scores that calculate the numbers of different food items consumed by individuals or households over a given period of time, for example, have been used as surrogate measures of the nutritional adequacy of diets (Schorr et al., 1972; Romero de Gwynn and Sanjur, 1974; Sanjur and Romero, 1975; Caliendo et al., 1976). The measures are even more accurate predictors, particularly of infants' and children's nutritional status, when certain protein factors, like milk, are weighted (Romero de Gwynn and Sanjur, 1974).
Food diversity indices may also be useful for understanding why, in certain cultural contexts, children undereat and suffer malnutrition in spite of apparently adequate supplies of the staple grain or tuber. Sometimes malnutrition occurs because children's diets are too boring as well as too bulky (Wilson and Wilson, 1971; Gershoff et al., 1977). Food diversity indices may also be developed as a tool to monitor how availability of variety influences food choices and, therefore, levels of consumption of those who overeat
As yet, we have no inkling of the interactions between dietary variety, relative perceptions, and affective values of and intakes of particular foods, especially those that are sweet and fatty. This may be a promising area for research aimed at control of overeating (Drewnowski and Greenwood, 1983). In modern industrial societies, where people are exposed to very large amounts and very great varieties of foods, we need to have new ways to control intake, since the satiety that accompanies habitual exposure to a very monotonous diet is absent (Mead, 1964). Analysis of factors such as novelty, satiety, and complexity can also contribute to studies of food acceptance and preference (Koster, 1981).
Dietary complexity might also be used to roughly screen households for malnutrition within a poor community where increased complexity indicates a diet with greater variety and, consequently, superior nutritional value. DeWalt et al. (1979), by contrast, used food complexity scores as one of several ways to compare diets of rich and poor, both qualitatively and nutritionally, in a Mexican community. Particular aspects of dietary complexity, such as demand for a minimum level of variety or for a certain number of "fatty" meal components at different socio-economic levels, might be worked into linear programming models of dietary construction (Caravan, 1976). However, for purposes of anticipating nutritional outcome, rules of community food redistribution and intra-household distribution of food may be more significant.
Social Factors in Diet Selection
Food distribution and sharing are discussed at length in the chapter by J. Goode in this volume. At the group level, food-sharing rules are one of the means by which people in hunting and gathering and horticultural societies maintain co-operation for other pursuits. Food-sharing rules are also one of the ways to assure systematic distribution of environmental resources from fortunate to less fortunate groups and individuals. Wiessner (1981), for example, has shown how !Kung San continue to operate partnerships that include food exchange or opportunities for work even under the conditions of the substantially changed cultural and economic environments of government-settled communities.
Even in complex societies, food sharing has its political and nutritional dimensions, as in the institutionalized food-sharing that goes on during feasting. Diskin (1978) and Greenberg (1981) have suggested that high-quality protein and vitamin-rich foods distributed during the cycles of festivals in the rural Mexican villages they studied may make substantial contributions to the diets of poor participants. Furthermore, the timing of the festivals seems to have been such that food distribution took place during periods when the population was most in need of protein and nutrients. Future studies on the question of the importance of food sharing might try to account for: (1) how much and what kinds of foods are prepared; (2) who receives the food (what is their socio-economic status); (3) what percentage of people of a particular socio-economic status are served for how many days per year, especially during the lean season, when seasonal hunger is a problem; and (4) how these customs are attenuated in (a) times of scarcity or (b) times of socio-economic change.
A related set of questions is concerned with what rules exist for food sharing under conditions of dearth. Various ethnographic cases indicate that, as people confront potential starvation conditions, they avoid their obligations to share by hiding and hoarding food. Most hospitality obligations are triggered when food is available. During famines, people may lie about their food supply, saying that they have nothing to offer, but later they eat alone (Richards, 1939; Holmberg, 1950). Generosity may shrink to immediate, instead of extended, kin networks (Firth, 1959; Dirks, 1978). Or, food sharing may be eliminated altogether, even that usually found between close kin such as parents and young children (Turnbull, 1972). Probably observation is the only certain way to collect information on such questions; yet in situations of dearth, the anthropologist or nutritionist also faces an ethical dilemma of "just watching" while people go hungry. Usually under such conditions, co-operation in social relations within the society will also decrease. Brady and Laughlin (1978) have suggested that the extent of working together in social relations can be quantified under different conditions of resource availability, although it may be difficult to arrive at quantified values for both resources and co-operation that will provide meaningful cross-cultural comparisons.
Finally, Douglas and Gross (1981) have offered a method for combining quantitative measures of the rules of social relations and structuredness in diet. They hope, in the long run, to be able to quantify both society and diet to show how disruption of social relations (social breakdown) leads to regular changes in social nutritional systems (aspects of dietary complexity).
Each of the foregoing studies suggests additional ways to conceptualize and quantify the factors in social organizations that contribute to nutritional well-being. Such factors are rarely captured by correlations of single social or cultural factors with nutritional status, since the nutritional well-being of individuals is the result of the interaction of numerous factors operating within the household and the community, such as the social organization of food production and food preparation, rules for distribution of food within the household, and regulations for eating and exchange with members of other households. Only by considering such social factors, in combination with other cultural and psychological aspects of food habits and environmental factors impinging on health status, will a more accurate picture of the sources of malnutrition be drawn.
This chapter has reviewed the factors and structures that anthropologists, as well as other social scientists and nutritionists, have identified as affecting food selection, food preferences, and dietary intakes. There is always the problem in describing such dietary features of further identifying the proper level at which to address an investigation. Sensory and cultural symbolic factors are usually arrived at by interviewing and observing individuals, and then describing and quantifying their responses as a cultural or community description, within which one may also describe variations. Conversely, there is the problem of working backwards from data on dietary intake or "cultural rules" to individual reasons for choosing to eat particular foods in particular patterns. Anthropological research allows us to decipher the structure of meals and, more generally, diets, and discuss the attributes of foods that are selected within them. But there have been few studies, none of them convincing, that demonstrate the relative weight of the different factors operating in the food choices of individuals, and how these might lead to predictions of consumption patterns at the community level, checked by actual observations or recall of dietary intakes.
We are left, then, with a compendium of methodologies by which to measure the economic, social symbolic, perceived nutritional, and other factors that enter into dietary construction and selection of individual foods. But to elucidate the relative weighting in food choices, we must rely on largely anecdotal information, culled from observations of dietary intakes in conjunction with conversations of people explaining why they chose the foods they did. As indicated in the sections above, the greatest weight in situations of limited food or monetary resources seems to be the cost various foods, in conjunction with their perceived values as filling.
Probably the best natural settings in which to study such dietary decision-making would be those in which new foods, especially new staple starches, are being introduced. For example, where there has been an attempt to introduce a new variety of grain or a different species similar to the old from the scientist's perspective, important questions may be the relative weights people put on traditional concepts of "human food" versus "animal food" (in some cases, the traditional designation of the new introduction) the taste, colour, and texture of the two variables, and their relative costs per unit.
Only through such investigations, combining interviews on the cultural qualities attributed to different foods and observations of their actual consumption, will nutritionists and anthropologists be able to ascertain the relative weighting of factors that contribute to food selection in particular cultural cases. As suggested above, it is probably best to proceed in such an analysis by first identifying the major local categories of foods and food expenditures, e.g. the major staple grain or tuber, the principal sauces or relishes, and the spices and condiments that may form part of them or be extra purchases, and then examine the inter-substitutions of different items within such categories, and the relative weighting (in caloric intake and cost) between such categories in different households within a community and between communities.
Such studies in the short and long term can certainly contribute to our understanding of the effects of advertising and promotions on consumption of particular adult foods and infant formula foods, and our ability to predict the possible impact of changing food-price policy by governments. Both the theoretical and applied studies of socio-cultural factors in human food selection will benefit by such advances in research.
Ahern, E. 1975. Sacred and Secular Medicine in a Taiwan Village: A Study of Cosmological Disorders. In: National Institutes of Health (NIH), Medicine in Chinese Culture, pp. 91-113. US Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C.
Amerine, M. A. 1966. Flavor as a Value. In: S. M. Farber, N. L. Wilson, and R. H. L. Wilson, eds.. Food and Civilization. Charles C. Thomas, Chicago, III.
Autret, M. M. 1962. Comment on the Social Psychology of Food Habits. In: A. Burgess and R. F. A. Dean, eds., Malnutrition and Food Habits. Macmillan, New York.
Barker, L. M., ed. 1982. The Psychobiology of Human Food Selection. Avi Publishing Co., Westport, Conn.
Barthes, R. 1975. Toward a Psychosociology of Contemporary Food Consumption. In: E. Forster and R. Forster, eds., European Diet from Pre-industrial to Modern Times. Harper Torchbooks, New York.
Bartoshuk, L. M. 1975. Taste Quality. In: T. Silverstone, ea., Dahlem Workshop Reports: Appetite and Food Intake. Life Sciences Research Report, 2. Verlag Chemie, Deerfield Beach, Fla. . 1978. The Psychophysics of Taste. Am. J. Clin. Nutr., 31:1068-1077.
Beck, B. 1969. Colour and Heat in South Indian Ritual. Man, 4: 553-572.
Beller, A. S. 1977. Fat and Thin: A Natural History of Obesity. McGraw-Hill, New York.
Bressani, R. 1983. Effect of Chemical Changes during Storage on the Nutritional Quality of Common Beans. Food Nutr. Bull., 5(1):23-24.
Brady, 1. A., and C. D. Laughlin. 1978. Introduction: Diaphasis and Change in Human Populations. In: C. D. Laughlin and 1. Brady, eds., Extinction and Survival in Human Populations. Columbia University Press, New York.
Calavan, M. M. 1976. A Model for Food Selection in Northern Thailand. Ecol. Food Nutr., 5: 6374.
Caliendo, M. A., D. Sanjur, J. Wright, and G. Cummings. 1976. Use of PATH Analysis as a Statistical Technique for the Analysis of Qualitative Nutritional Data. Ecol. Food Nutr., 5: 7581.
Calloway, D., and J. C. Gibbs. 1976. Food Patterns and Food Assistance Programs in the Cocopah Indian Community. Ecol. Food Nutr., 5:183-196.
Colson. A. B. 1976. Binary Oppositions and the Treatment of Sickness among Akawaif. In: J. Louden, ea., Social Anthropology and Medicine. Academic Press. New York.
Conklin. H. C. 1954. The Relation of the Hanunoo Culture to the Plant World. Ph.D. dissertation. Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Cosman, M. P. 1976. Fabulous Feasts: Medieval Cookery and Ceremony. George Braziller, New York.
Cosminsky, S. 1977. Alimento and Fresco: Nutritional Concepts and Their Implications for Health Care. Human Organization, 36:203.
Curtis, K. 1983. I Can Never Go Anywhere Empty-handed. Food Exchange and Reciprocity in an Italian-American Community. Ph.D. dissertation. Temple University, Philadelphia, Pa.
Desor, J. A., L. S. Greene, and O. Maller. 1975. Preference in Sweet and Salty in 9- to 15-yearold Adult Humans. Science, 190: 686-687.
DeWalt, K. M. 1983. Dietary Strategies in Several Ecological Zones of Southern Honduras. Paper presented at the 82nd annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association Symposium on Continuity and Change: Dietary Modernization in Latin America, Chicago, 111., 17 November.
DeWalt, K., and G. Pelto. 1976. Food Use and Household Ecology in a Mexican Community. In: T. Fitzgerald, ea., Nutrition and Anthropology in Action, pp. 79-93. Van Gorcum, Assen, Netherlands.
DeWalt. K. M., P. B. Kelley, and G. H. Pelto. 1979. Nutritional Correlates of Economic Micro-differentiation in a Highland Mexican Community. In: N. Jerome. R. Kandel, and G. Pelto, eds., Nutritional Anthropology, pp. 205-221. Redgrave, Pleasantville, N.Y.
Dirks. R. 1978. Resource Fluctuations and Competitive Transformations in West Indian Slave Societies. In: C. D. Laughlin and 1. Brady, eds., Extinction and Survival in Human Populations. Columbia University Press, New York.
Diskin, M. 1978. Discussion. Symposium on Mexican Food Systems. 77th annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Los Angeles, Calif.
Douglas, M. 1966. Purity and Danger. Penguin, Baltimore, Md.
_____. 1972. Deciphering a Meal. Daedalus, 101:61-81.
Douglas, M., and J. Gross. 1981. Food and Culture: Measuring the Intricacy of Rule Systems. Soc. Sci. Inform., 20: 1-35.
Drewnowski, A., and M. R. C. Greenwood. 1983. Cream and Sugar: Human Preferences for High Fat Foods. Physiology and Behavior, 30: 629-633.
Etkin, N. L., and P. J. Ross. 1982. Food as Medicine and Medicine as Food. An Adaptive Framework for the Interpretation of Plant Utilization among the Hausa of Northern Nigeria. Soc. Sci. Med., 16: 155-173.
Firth, R. 1959. Social Change in Tikopia. Allen & Unwin, London.
______.1966. Housekeeping among Malay Peasants, 2nd ed. London School of Economics Monographs on Social Anthropology, no. 7. Humanities Press, Inc., New York.
Fischer, J. L., A. Fischer, and F. Mahoney. 1977. Totemism and Allergy. In: D. Landy, ea., Culture, Disease, and Healing, pp. 154-159. Macmillan, New York.
Flannery, K. V. 1973. The Origins of Agriculture. Ann. Rev. Anthropol.. 2: 271-310.
Fortune, R. F. 1953. Relationships between Spanish and Spanish-American Folk Medicine. J. Am. Folklore, 66: 201 -217.
Geertz, C. 1960. The Religion of Java. The Free Press, Glencoe, III.
Gershoff, S. N. 1971. Childhood Malnutrition in the United States. In: S. Margen and N. L. Wilson, eds., Progress in Human Nutrition, vol. 1, pp. 68-74. Avi Press, Westport, Conn.
Gershoff, S. N., R. B. McGandy, Suttapreyasri, et al. 1977. Nutrition Studies in Thailand 11. Effects of Fortification of Rice with Lysine, Threonine, Thiamin, Riboflavin, Vitamin A, and Iron on Preschool Children. Am. 1. Clin. Nutr., 30: 1185- 1195.
Gopaldas, T., N. Srinivasan, and F. Vardarasan. 1975. Project Poshak: An Integrated Health and Nutrition Macro/Pilot Study of Preschool Children in Rural Madhya Pradesh, vols. I and 2. CARE India, New Delhi.
Greenberg, J. 1981. Santiago's Sword. University of California Press, Berkeley, Calif.
Harrison, G. 1975. Primary Adult Lactase Deficiency: A Problem in Anthropological Genetics. Am. Anthropol., 77: 812-835.
Harwood, A. 1971. The Hot-Cold Theory of Disease: Implications for Treatment of Puerto Rican Patients. J. Am. Med. Assoc., 216: 1153-1158
Hayden, B. 1981. Omnivorous Primates. In: R. S. O. Harding and G. Teleki, eds., Subsistence and Ecological Adaptations of Modern Hunter-gatherers, pp. 334-421. Columbia University Press, New York.
Heiser, C. 1981. From Seed to Civilization, 2nd ed. H. Freeman, San Francisco, Calif.
Holmberg, A. R. 1950. Nomads of the Long Bow. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington, D.C.
Jerome, N. W. 1977. Taste Experience and the Development of a Dietary Preference for Sweet in Humans: Ethnic and Cultural Variations in Early Taste Experience. Taste and Development: The Genesis of Sweet Preference. US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare Publication (HEW), 77:1068. HEW, Washington, D.C.
Jerome, N. 1979. Diet and Acculturation: The Case of Black American Immigrants. In: N. W. Jerome, F. F. Kandel, and G. H. Pelto, eds., Nutritional Anthropology, pp. 275-325. Redgrave, Pleasantville, N.Y.
Jochim, M. 1981.. Strategies for Survival. Academic Press, New York.
Johnson, A. 1980. The Limits of Formalism in Agricultural Decision Research. In: P. F. Bartlett, ea., Agricultural Decision-making, pp. 19-43. Academic Press, New York.
Johnson, A. and M. Baksh. 1983. Ecological and Structural Influences on the Proportions of Wild Food in the Diets of Two Machiguenga Communities. Paper presented at the Wenner-Gren Symposium No. 94, Food Preferences and Aversions, Cedar Key, Fla., 23-30 October.
Kandel, R. F., and G. F. Pelto. 1979. The Health Food Movement. In: N. W. Jerome, R. F. Kandel, and G. H. Pelto, eds., Nutritional Anthropology, pp. 325-363. Redgrave, Pleasantville NY.
Kaplan, L. 1954. The Cultivated Bean of the Prehistoric Southwest. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden, 43: 189-247.
Kolasa, K. 1978. I Won't Cook Turnip Greens If You Won't Cook Kielbasa: Food Behaviour in Polonia and Its Health Implications. In: E. E. Banwens, ea., The Anthropology of Health, pp. 130-140. C. V. Mosby, St. Louis, Mo.
Koster, E. P. 1981. Time and Frequency Analysis: A New Approach to the Measurement of Some Less Well-known Aspects of Food Preferences. In: I. Solms and R. H. Hall, eds., Criteria of Food Acceptance: How Man (Chooses What He Eats, pp. 240-252. Forster, Zurich.
Lee, R. 1968. What Hunters Do for a Living, or, How to Make Out on Scarce Resources. In: R. Lee and 1. DeVore, eds., Man the Hunter, pp. 30-48. Aldine, Chicago, III.
Lewin, K. 1943. Forces behind Food Habits and Methods of Change. The Problem of Changing Food Habits, pp. 35-65. Bulletin of the National Research Council, 108. Committee on Food Habits, National Research Council, Washington, D.C.
Logan, M. 1977. Anthropological Research on the Hot-Cold Theory of Disease: Some Methodological Suggestions. Med. Anthropol., 1(4): 87-112.
Malinowski, B. 1935. Coral Gardens and Their Magic: A Study of the Methods of Tilling the Soil and of Agricultural Rites in the Trobriand Islands. American Book Company, New York.
Manderson, L. 1981. Traditional Food Classifications and Humoral Medical Theory in Peninsular Malaysia. Ecol. Food Nutr., 11: 81-93.
Manderson, L., and M. Mathews. 1981. Vietnamese Behavioral and Dietary Precautions during Pregnancy. Ecol. Food Nutr., 11: 1-8.
Massara, E. B. 1980. Obesity and Cultural Weight Valuations: A Puerto Rican Case. Appetite, 1: 291-298.
Mathews, M., and L. Manderson. 1981. Vietnamese Behavioral and Dietary Precautions during Confinement. Ecol. Food Nutr., 11:9-16.
Mazess, R. B. 1971. Hot-Cold Beliefs in Peru. J. Amer. Diet. Assoc., 53: 109.
McCullough, J. M. 1973. Human Ecology, Heat Adaptation, and Belief Systems: The Hot-Cold System of Yucatan. Southwest J. Anthropol., 29: 32-39.
Mead, M. 1964. Food Habits Research: Problems of the 1960s. National Academy of the Sciences/National Research Council Publication, 1225. National Academy of the Sciences, Washington, D.C.
Messer, E. 1976a. The Ecology of a Vegetarian Diet in a Modernizing Mexican Community. In: T. K. Fitzgerald, ea., Nutrition and Anthropology in Action, pp. 117-124. Van Gorcum, Assen, Netherlands.
_______. 1976b. The Pragmatics of Folk Classification. Mich. Discuss. Anthropol., 2: 91-106.
_______. 1978. Zapotec Plant Knowledge: Classification, Uses, and Communications about Plants in Mitla, Oaxaca, Mexico. Memoirs of the Museum of Anthropology, 10. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Mich.
_______.1981. Hot-Cold Classifications: Theoretical and Practical Implications of a Mexican Study. Soc. Sci. Med., 15B: 133-145.
_______. 1986. Some Like It Sweet: Estimating Sweetness Preferences and Sucrose Intakes from Ethnographic and Experimental Data. Amer. Anthropologist, 88: 637-647.
Ogbu, J. M. 1973. Seasonal Hunger in Tropical Africa as a Cultural Phenomenon. Africa, 13: 317-332.
O'Laughlin, B. 1974. Mediation of Contradiction: Why Mbum Women Do Not Eat Chicken. In: M. Rosaldo and L. Lamphere, eds., Women, Culture, and Society, pp. 301-318. Stanford University Press, Stanford, Calif.
Omawale. 1980. Nutrition Problems Identification and Development Policy Implications. Ecol. Food Nutr., 9:113-122.
Passin, H., and J. W. Bennett. 1943. Social Process and Dietary Change. The Problem of Changing Food Habits. Bulletin of the National Research Council, 108. Committee on Food Habits, National Research Council, Washington, D.C.
Pelto, P. J., and G. H. Pelto. 1983. Nutrition, Health and Culture. In: D. L. Romanucci-Ross, E. E. Moerman, and R. Tancredi, eds., The Anthropology of Medicine, pp. 173-200. J. F. Bergin, New York.
Popkin, B. 1980. Time Allocation of the Mother and Child Nutriture. Ecol. Food Nutr., 9: 1-14.
Popkin B., and F. S. Solon. 1976. Income, Time, and the Working Mother and Nutriture. J. Trop. Pediatr. Env. Child Health, 6:156-166.
Reichel-Dolmatoff, G. 1968. Amazonian Cosmos. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 111.
Reutlinger, S. 1982. 1981-1982. World Bank Research on the Hunger Dimension of the Food Problem. World Bank Research News, 3(1): 3-9.
_______. N.d. More Effective Food Aid to the World's Poor and Hungry: A Fresh Look at Public Law 480, Title 11, Food Aid. Unpublished manuscript.
Richards, A. F. 1939. Land, Labour, and Diet in Northern Rhodesia: An Economic Study of the Bemba Tribe. Routledge, London.
Ritenbaugh, C. 1978. Human Foodways: A Window on Evolution. In: E. Banwens, ea., Anthropology and Health, pp. 11-120. C. V. Mosby, St Louis, Mo.
Romero de Gwynn, E., and D. Sanjur. 1974. Nutritional Anthropometry: Diet and Health-related Correlates among Preschool Children in Bogota, Colombia. Ecol. Food Nutr., 3: 273282.
Rotenberg, R. 1981. The Impact of Industrialization on Meal Patterns in Vienna, Austria. Ecol. Food Nutr., 11: 25-35.
Rozin, E. 1973. The Flavour Principle. Hawthorn, New York.
Rozin, P. 1976. The Selection of Foods by Rats, Humans, and Other Animals. Adv. Study Beh., 6: 21-76.
_______.1982. Human Food Selection: The Interaction of Biology, Culture, and Individual Experience. In: L. Barker, ea., The Psychobiology of Human Food Selection. pp. 225-254. Avi Publishing Co., Westport, Conn.
Rozin, P., and A E. Fallon. 1981. The Acquisition of Likes and Dislikes for Foods. In: J. Solms and R. L. Hall, eds., Criteria of Food Acceptance. How Man Chooses What He Eats, pp. 35-45. Forster, Zurich.
Rozin, E., and P. Rozin. 1981. Some Surprisingly Unique Characteristics of Human Food Preferences. In: A. Fenton and T. M. Owen, eds., Food in Perspective. Proceedings of the 34th International Conference on Ethnological Food Research, Cardiff, Wales, pp. 243-252. John Donal, Edinburgh.
Rozin. P., and D. Schiller. 1980. The Nature and Acquisition of a Preference for Chili Pepper by Humans. Motiv. Emotion, 4: 77- 101.
Sanjur, D., and E. Romero. 1975. Conceptual Levels of Dietary Indicators as Predictors of Nutritional Status. Nutr., 2: 214-222.
Schorr, B., D. Sanjur, and E. Erickson. 1972. Teen-age Food Habits: A Multidimensional Analysis. J. Am. Diet. Assoc.. 61:4.
Schuftan, C. 1979. Household Purchasing-power Deficit: A More Operational Indicator to Express Malnutrition. Ecol. Food Nutr., 8: 29-35.
Seeger, A. 1981. Nature and Society in Central Brazil: The Suya Indians of Mato Grosso. Cambridge. Mass.
Shack, D. N. 1969. Nutritional Processes and Personality Development among the Gurage. Ethnology, 8: 292-300.
Shack, W. 1971. Hunger, Anxiety, and Ritual: Deprivation and Spirit Possession among the Gurage of Ethiopia. Man (London), 69(1): 30-43.
Simoons, F. J. 1960. Northwest Ethiopia: Peoples and Economy. University of Wisconsin Press, Madison, Wis.
_______. 1973. The Determinants of Dairying and Milk Use in the Old World: Ecological, Physiological, and Cultural. Ecol. Food Nutr., 2: 83-90.
Sims, L. S. 1978. Food-related Value Orientations, Attitudes, and Beliefs of Vegetarians and Nonvegetarians. Ecol. Food Nutr., 7: 23-35.
Solms, J., and R. L. Hall, eds. 1981. Criteria of Food Acceptance: How Man Chooses What He Eats. Forster, Zurich.
Swain, T. 1976. Angiosperm-Reptile Co-evolution. In: A. D. A. Bellarts and C. B. Cox, eds., Morphology and Biology of Reptiles, pp. 107-122. Academic Press, New York.
Swantz, M. L. 1975. Socioeconomic Causes of Malnutrition in Moshi District. Research Paper No. 38, Bureau of Resource Assessment and Land Use Planning. University of Dar es Salaam, Tanzania.
Theophano, J. 1982. It's Really Tomato Sauce but We Can Cal] It Gravy: A Study of Food and Women's Work among Italian-American Families. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Pennsylvania, Philadephia, Pa.
Turnbull, C. 1972. The Mountain People. Simon & Schuster, New York.
Van Veen, M. S. 1971. Some Ecological Considerations of Nutritional Problems on Java. Ecol. Food Nutr., 1: 25-38.
Veith, l. 1949. The Yellow Emperor's Classic of Internal Medicine. University of California Press, Berkeley, Calif.
Wallace, A. F. C., and R. E. Ackerman. 1960. An Interdisciplinary Approach to Mental Disorder among the Polar Eskimos of North-west Greenland. Anthropologica New Series, 2: 249260.
Wiessner, P. 1981. Measuring the Impact of Social Ties on Nutritional Status among the !Kung San. Soc. Sci Inform., 415: 641-678.
Wilson, N. L., and R. H. L. Wilson. 1971. Dysnutrition and Boredom. In: S. Margen and N. L. Wilson, eds., Progress in Human Nutrition, vol. I, pp. 111-117. Avi Publishing Co, Westport, Conn.
Worseley, A. 1980. Thought for Food: Investigations of Cognitive Aspects of Food. Ecol. Food Nutr., 9: 65-80.
Young, J. C. 1981. Medical Choice in a Mexican Village. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, N.J.
Young, M. 1971. Fighting with Food. Cambridge University Press, New York.