Contents - Previous - Next


Concurrent experiments in animals


Adult animal studies of concurrent zinc deficiency and behavior typically employed a severe deprivation and demonstrated lethargy and poorer behavioral performance (Gordon et al. l 982, Hesse et al. l 979). Few studies of the concurrent effects of zinc deficiency on behavior of immature animals were located.

Strobel and Sandstead (1984) provided a preliminary report of studies using severe concurrent zinc deficiency (< 1D g Zn/g) in nursing and juvenile rhesus monkeys. Zinc-deprived nursing infants were more protected by their dams and more sensitive to maternal separation. Zinc-deprived juveniles had difficulties with retention of previously learned visual discrimination problems and with acquisition of more difficult problems of this type. Macapinlac et al. l 1967) induced a dietary zinc deficiency syndrome in immature squirrel monkeys and described behavioral changes but did not include formal behavioral assessments.

We recently reported a study of concurrent zinc deprivation on behavioral performance in juvenile rhesus monkeys (25-30 mo of age, sexual maturation 36-48 mo of age) using a crossover design (Golub et al. 1994) (see Table 2). A 2-D g Zn/g diet (moderate zinc deprivation) was fed over a 15-wk period and led to lowered plasma zinc levels but no inhibition of growth. The zinc deprivation period was compared with similar zinc-adequate (50 D g Zn/g) diet period that either preceded or followed it in two different subgroups. Spontaneous motor activity was lower during the zinc deprivation period. Performance of a visual attention task (the continuous performance test) was also poorer during the zinc deprivation period. A short-term memory task (delayed spatial alternation, 0-, 3- and 5-s delays) was affected only at the intermediate 3-s delay. It should be noted that behavioral effects were detected as changes from individual baselines and often represented a failure to improve rather than a deterioration of performance.

Developmental/concurrent studies in monkeys


Over a period of several years we conducted a series of studies of rhesus monkeys deprived continuously of zinc (marginal deprivation) from conception through adolescence (see Table 2). The studies included behavioral assessments at several stages of maturation. This experimental paradigm provides concurrent deprivation preceded by developmental deprivation during the period of rapid brain growth, the most probable situation in human-malnourished populations. However, this paradigm does not allow separation of concurrent and critical periods effects.

Transient growth retardation was seen in infancy and adolescence, but the growth, health and maturation of the animals were not grossly affected. Hypotonia, recorded at birth, may have been secondary to previously documented effects of zinc deficiency on labor and delivery. Results of the behavioral assessments from this study are outlined in Table 2. In general hypoactivity-reduced environmental responsiveness was characteristic of the young monkeys. Effects on performance of a cognitive task (visual discrimination learning and reversal) seemed more marked during the adolescent than the juvenile period. Performance of a spatial-delayed response task was improved, a finding that is consistent with reduced activity. Hypogeusia and altered preference for novel foods were demonstrated, but reduced food intake was not recorded at any time in the study.

Studies in humans


Adult studies of behavioral effects of concurrent zinc deprivation include observations (Sandstead et al. 1981) and, more recently, structured tests of cognitive function (Penland 1991, Tucker and Sandstead 1984, Wallwork et al. 1982). Several behavioral measures, including some reflecting memory, were found to be sensitive to zinc deprivation, but data were too limited to draw firm conclusions. Adult studies may be relevant to concurrent effects in children; there is no reason a priori to assume that concurrent effects in children would be fundamentally different from those seen adults.

There are no strictly experimental studies of zinc deficiency and behavior in infants or children. Two studies with zinc supplementation in school-age children were done by the same research group, one in Canada and one in Guatemala. Both studies supplemented 6 to 7-y-old children with 10 mg Zn/d. In the Canadian study, boys were selected for low height-for-age centiles and received either placebo or supplement for 12 mo (Gibson et al. 1989). In the Guatemalan study, poorly nourished children (male and female) were randomly assigned to receive placebo or the zinc supplement for 25 d (Cavan et al. 1 993a). Both studies used multiple indices of zinc status and found a positive response to supplement in terms of height-for-age (Ontario) and body composition (Guatemala). In both studies, supplemented children failed to show improvement on subtests of the Detroit Test of Learning Abilities selected to assess attention.

A number of behavioral assessments (standardized cognitive testing, observation and teacher ratings of classroom behavior, activity level and social and emotional measures) were determined in school children (age 7-10) from an Egyptian village where the traditional diet had a low level of bioavailable zinc (Wachs et al. 1995). There was a significant correlation between dietary zinc and girls' attention-seeking behavior in the classroom.

Dietary zinc, along with six other nutrients, provided significant prediction of this behavior in multivariate regressions. Dietary zinc in combination with five other nutrient variables also significantly predicted boys' activity level in multiple regressions.

Unfortunately, behavior has not as yet been studied in connection with zinc status or zinc intake of infants and adolescents. These groups are at higher risk of zinc deficiency due to rapid growth than are school-age 15 to 12-y-old) children.

Mechanisms of zinc deprivation effects on behavior


A major emphasis in developmental zinc deprivation research has been on the role of zinc-dependent enzymes in critical cell replication processes and consequent effects on brain growth. Indeed, linear growth is sensitive to zinc deprivation, age groups experiencing rapid growth are most susceptible to zinc deficiency and tissues that require continuous cell replication (skin, immune system) produce the most striking symptoms of zinc deficiency. However, in the case of the brain, where extensive cell replication takes place only during early development, other functions of zinc require more attention (see Table 3). As focus shifts from critical periods and brain growth in malnutrition research, a similar change is appropriate for research on developmental zinc deficiency and behavior.

The role of zinc in brain has received systematic study only within the past 10 y. As described extensively in a review by Frederickson (1989), zinc is important to the function of a number of enzymes and other proteins, including some unique to brain and important to neurotransmission. In addition, zinc functions in membrane stabilization and permeability. Also, as noted earlier, the mossy fiber system of the hippocampus contains vesicular zinc that appears to function in connection with specific neurotransmitters. However, it is not clear at this time what, if any, influence dietary zinc deprivation has on the function of zinc-dependent systems in brain.

TABLE 3
Potential mechanisms of zinc deprivation effects on behavior

Roles of zinc in the CNS

• Protein structure |zinc finger)

• Enzyme activity (catalytic site)

• Neurotransmitter action (ligand gated ion channels)

• Hippocampal function (mossy fiber system)

Extra-CNS influences on CNS function

• Neurotransmitter precursor production (liver)

• Hormone/growth factor transport and receptor binding

• Receptor binding (GH, NGF)

• Hormone/toxicant metabolism (liver, testes)

• Energy supply (pancreatic insulin production)

Indirect influences on CNS function

• Adrenocortical activation due to starvation

• Altered tissue trace metal content, especially copper

• Smaller body size due to reduced food intake/growth

• Selective mortality

It is important to note that there is considerable brain sparing as regards zinc homeostasis. Brain turnover is slow, intracellular zinc concentrations are maintained by an active uptake process and zinc is tightly bound in metalloenzymes and structural proteins (zinc fingers) (Frederickson 1989). In addition, a unique zinc-inducible metallothionein has been found in the brain (Soumillion et al. 1 992,) which presumably plays a homeostatic role. Indeed, only a few dietary zinc deprivation regimens proved effective in reducing brain zinc even for short time periods. Brain zinc was lowered 20 and 37% in 1 5-d-old mouse pups deprived of zinc from d 15 gestation; however, this effect was not seen at 10 or 21 d of age (Golub et al. 1986). Hippocampal zinc staining was reduced in severely growth retarded 18 to 20-d-old rat pups deprived of zinc throughout lactation (Dreosti et al. 1981). Thus, zinc deprivation and zinc deficiency cannot be assumed to be reflected in brain zinc status. This lends emphasis to examination of mediation of behavioral change by non-CNS systems that secondarily impact brain.

Caveats for integrating conclusions from animal and human studies


Animal studies produce zinc deficiency in isolation from any other nutritional deprivation and under optimal health and environmental conditions. Precise and consistent levels of deprivation for predetermined periods of time are used. This is in contrast to the situation in malnourished children in which multiple deficiencies of varying extent exist for undetermined periods during development. The advantage of animal studies is that direct, causal relationships can be determined, in contrast to human studies, in which inference about causation is limited. Supplement studies in children provide a stronger basis for causal inference; however, they are limited to understanding the causal factors in rehabilitation, not in etiology (Golden 1991). In this sense, animal studies can be seen as providing basic information about how single nutrient deficiency affects behavioral development and how to prevent the adverse effects of deprivation, whereas human studies tell more about how single nutrient deficiencies fit into the constellation of factors determining behavioral well-being of children and how valuable single nutrient supplements are in improving the behavioral well-being of deficient children. Both types of studies are necessary and valuable, but integration of findings must be undertaken with an appreciation for the different aims and designs of these types of research.

Acknowledgments


The authors thank their colleagues associated with the dietary zinc deprivation project at the California Regional Primate Research Center for continuing support and input.

Literature cited


Allen, L. H. (1993) Nutritional influences on linear growth: a general review. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 47: 1-15.

Buzina, R., Jusic, M., Sapunar, J. & Milanovic, N. (1980) Zinc nutrition and taste acuity in school children with impaired growth. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 33: 2262-2267.

Caldwell, D. F. Oberleas, D. & Prasad, A. S. (1973) Reproductive performance of chronic mildly zinc deficient rats and the effects on behavior of their offspring Nutr. Rep. Int. 7: 309-319

Cannon, D. S., Crawford, l. L. & Carrell, L. E. (1988) Zinc deficiency conditions food aversions in rats. Physiol. Behav 42: 245-247

Catalanotto, F. A. (1978) The trace metal zinc and taste. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 31: 1098-1103.

Cavan, K. R., Gibson, R. S., Grazioso, C. F., Isalgue, A M, Ruz, M. & Solomons, N. W. (1993a) Growth and body composition of periurban Guatemalan children in relation to zinc status: a cross-sectional study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.57: 334-343.

Cavan, K. R., Gibson, R S., Grazioso, C F., Isalgue, A. M., Ruz, M. & Solomons, N W. (1993b) Growth and body composition of periurban Guatemalan children in relation to zinc status: a longitudinal zinc intervention trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr 57: 344352

Chafetz, M D., Abshire, F. M. & Bernard, D L. (1984) Zinc deficiency in adult rats alters foraging in a radial maze. In: The Neurobiology of Zinc (Frederickson, C. J. et al., eds), pp. 109120. Alan R. Liss, New York, NY.

Chesters, J. K & Quarterman, J. (1970) Effects of zinc deficiency on food intake and feeding patterns of rats. Br. J. Nutr. 24: 1061 1069.

Christensen, C. M., Caldwell, D. F. & Oberleas, D. (1974) Establishment of a learned preference for a zinc-containing solution by zinc-deficient rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 87: 415-421.

Dahlstrom, K. A., Ament, M. E., Medhin, M. G. & Meurling, S. (1986) Serum trace elements in children receiving long-term parenteral nutrition. J. Pediatr. 109: 625-630.

Dreosti, l. E., Manuel, S. J., Buckley, R. A., Fraser, F. J. & Record, 1. R. (1981) The effect of late prenatal and/or early postnatal zinc deficiency on the development and some biochemical aspects of the cerebellum and hipppocampus in rats. Life Sci. 28: 21332141.

Essatara, M. B., Morley, J. E., Levine, A. S., Elson, M. K., Shafer, R. B. & McClain, C. J. (1984) The role of the endogenous opiates in zinc deficiency anorexia. Physiol. Behav. 32: 475-478

Favier, A. E. (1992) Hormonal effects of zinc on growth in children. Biol. Trace Element Res 32: 383-398.

Ferguson, E L., Gibson, R. S., Thompson, L. U. & Ounpuu, S. (1989) Dietary calcium, phytate, and zinc intakes and the calcium, phytate, and zinc molar ratios of the diets of a selected group of East African children. Am. J Clin. Nutr 50: 1450-1456

Frederickson, C. J. (1989) Neurobiology of zinc and zinc-containingneurons Int Rev. Neurobiol. 31: 146-238.

Ghavami-Maibodi, S. Z., Collipp, P. J., Castro-Magana, M., Stewart, C & Chen, S. Y. (1983) Effect of oral zinc supplements on growth, hormonal levels and zinc in healthy short children. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 27: 214-219.

Gibson, R. S., Heywood, A., Yaman, C., Sohlstrom, A., Thompson, L. U. & Heywood, P. (1991) Growth in children from the Wosera subdistrict, Papua New Guinea, in relation to energy and protein intakes and zinc status. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 53: 782789.

Gibson, R. S., Vanderkooy, P. D. S., MacDonald A. C., Goldman, A., Ryan, B. A. & Berry, M. (1989) A growth-limiting, mild zinc-deficiency syndrome in some South Ontario boys with low height percentiles. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 49: 1266-1273.

Golden, M. H. N. (1991) The nature of nutritional deficiency in relation to growth failure and poverty. Acta Paediatr. Scan Suppl. 374: 95-110.

Golub, M. S., Gershwin, M. E. & Hurley, L. S. (1984a) Studies of marginal zinc deprivation in rhesus monkeys. II. Pregnancy outcome Am J Clin Nutr. 39: 879-887.

Golub, M. S., Gershwin, M. E., Hurley, L. S. & Hendrickx, A. G. (1988) Studies of marginal zinc deprivation in rhesus monkeys. VIII. Effects on early adolescence. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 47: 10461051.

Golub, M. S., Gershwin, M. E., Hurley, L. S., Hendrickx, A. G. & Baly, D L. (1.982) Induction of marginal zinc deficiency in female rhesus monkeys. Am. J Primatol 3: 299-305.

Golub, M. S., Gershwin, M. E., Hurley, L. S. & Saito, W. Y. (1985) Studies of marginal zinc deprivation in rhesus monkeys. VII. Infant behavior. Am. J Clin. Nutr 42: 1229-1239.

Golub, M. S. Gershwin, M. E., Hurley, L. S., Saito, W. Y. & Hendrickx, A. G. (1984b) Studies of marginal zinc deprivation in rhesus monkeys. IV. Growth of infants in the first year. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 40: 1192-1202.

Golub, M. S., Gershwin, M. E. & Vijayan, V. K. (1983) Passive avoidance performance of mice fed marginally or severely zinc deficient diets during post-embryonic brain development. Physiol. Behav. 30: 409-413.

Golub, M. S., Keen, C. L., Gershwin, M. E. & Vijayan, V. K. (1986) Growth, development and brain zinc levels in mice marginally or severely deprived of zinc during postembryonic brain development Nutr. Behav. 3: 169-180

Golub, M. S., Keen, C L., Hendrickx, A. G. & Gershwin, M. E. (1990) Food preference of young rhesus monkeys fed marginally zinc deficient diets Primates 32: 49-59.

Golub, M. S., Takeuchi, P. T., Keen, C L., Gershwin, M. E, Hendrickx, A G. & Lonnerdal, B. (1994) Modulation of behavioral performance of prepubertal monkeys by moderate dietary zinc deprivation. Am J. Clin. Nutr. 60: 238-243.

Gordon, E. E F., Bond, R. C., Gordon, R. C. & Denny, M. R. (1982) Zinc deficiency and behavior: a developmental perspective. Physiol. Behav. 81: 893-897.

Greeley, S J (1984) Zinc repletion during late gestation following chronic suboptimal zinc intake. Nutr. Rep. Int 30: 389-395

Grider, A., Bailey, L. B. & Cousins, R. J. (1990) Erythrocyte metallothionein as an index of zinc status in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad Sci. U.S A 87: 1259-1262.

Halas, E. S. (1983) Behavioral changes accompanying zinc deficiency in animals. In: Neurobiology of the Trace Elements (Dreosti, l. E. & Smith, R. M, eds), Vol. I, Trace Element Neurobiology and Deficiencies, pp. 213-43. Humana Press, Clifton, NJ.

Halas, E. S., Burger, P. A. & Sandstead, H. H. (1980) Food motivation of rehabilitated malnourished rats: implications for (earning studies Anim Learn Behav. 8: 152-158.

Halas, E. S., Hanlon, M. J & Sandstead, H. H. (1975) Intrauterine nutrition and aggression. Nature 257: 221-222

Halas, E. S. Heinrich, M D & Sandstead, H H. (1979) Longterm memory deficits in adult rats due to postnatal malnutrition. Physiol Behav. 22: 991-997.

Halas, E S., Reynolds, G M. & Sandstead, H H. (1977) Intrauterine nutrition and its effects on aggression Physiol Behav. 19: 653661.

Halas, E S. & Sandstead, H. H. (1975) Some effects of prenatal zinc deficiency on behavior of the adult rat. Pediatr. Res. 9: 9497.

Halas, E. S. & Sandstead, H. H (1980) Short-term memory deficits in rats due to early malnutrition: a preliminary report. Fed. Proc. 40:839 (abs.).

Halas, E. S. & Sandstead, H. H. (1981) Short-term memory deficits in adult rats due to marginal zinc deficiency during gestation and lactation. Fed Proc. 40: 839 (abs.).

Halas, E. S. & Sandstead, H. H. (1982) Short-term memory (STM) deficits due to mild zinc deficiency during gestation and lactation: a reaffirmation. Fed Proc 41: 781 (abs.).

Halsted, J. A., Ronaghy, H. A, Abadi, P. Haghshenass, M, Amirhademi, G. H., Barakat, R. M. & Reinhold, J. G. (1972) Zinc deficiency in man: the Shiraz Experiment. Am. J. Med. 53: 277284

Hambidge, K M, Walravens, P. A., Jacobs, M. & Baum, J. D. (1971) Low levels of zinc in hair anorexia, poor growth and hypogeusia in children. Pediatr. Res. 6: 868-874.

Haynes, D. G., Golub, M S. Gershwin, M. E., Cheung, A T. W., Hurley, L. S. & Hendrickx, A G. (1987) Long-term marginal zinc deprivation in rhesus monkeys. l. Influence on adult female breeders prior to conception. Am. J Clin. Nutr 45: 1492- 1502.

Henkin, R.i (1984) Zinc in taste function: a critical review. Biol Trace Element Res. 6: 263-280.

Hesse, G. W., Hesse, K A F. & Catalanotto, F. A. (1979) Behavioral characteristics of rats experienring chronic zinc deficiency Physiol. Behav. 22: 211-215

Hunt, C. D., Halas, E. S. & Sandstead, H. H. (1984) Mild perinatal zinc deficiency affects brain hippocampal morphology and behavior. Fed. Proc. 43: 382 (abs.).

Hurley, L. S. & Swenerton, H. (1966) Congenital malformation resulting from zinc deficiency in:rats. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 123: 692-696.

Keen, C. L., Lönnerdal, B., Golub, M. S., Olin, K. L., Graham, T. W., Uriu-Hare, J., Hendrickx, A. G. & Gershwin, M. E. (1993) Effect of the severity of maternal zinc deficiency on pregnancy outcome and infant zinc status in rhesus monkeys. Pediatr. Res. 33: 233241.

Lask, B, Fosson, A., Rolfe, U. & Thomas, S. (1993) Zinc deficiency and childhood-onset anorexia nervosa. J Clin. Psychiatr. 54: 6366.

Lehti, K. K. (1990) Breast milk folic acid and zinc concentrations of lactating, low socioeconomic, Amazonian women and the effect of age and parity on the same two nutrients. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 44: 675-680.

Liu, X. W, Dejima, Y., Suzuki, T, Himeno, S & Okazaki, Y. (1991) Marginal zinc deficiency and changes in behavioral salt taste threshold and salt preference in mice J. Nutr. Sci. Vitaminol. 37: 185-199.

Lokken, P. M., Halas, E. S. & Sandstead, H. H (1973) Influence of zinc deficiency on behavior. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 144: 680-682.

Lönnerdal, B (1986) Effects of maternal dietary intake on human milk composition. J. Nutr 116: 499-513.

Luecke, R W (1984) Domestic animals in the elucidation of zinc's role in nutrition. Fed. Proc. 43: 2823-2828.

Macapinlac, M. P., Barney, G. H., Pearson, W. N. & Darby, W. J. (1967) Production of zinc deficiency in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). J Nutr .93: 499-510.

Masters, D. G., Keen, C. L., Lonnerdal, B. & Hurley, L. S. (1983) Zinc deficiency teratogenicity: the protective role of maternal tissue catabolism. J. Nutr. 21: 905-912.

Mattes, R. D. (1985) Gustation as a determinant of ingestion: methodological issues. Am. J Clin. Nutr. 41: 672-683.

Meadows, N J, Ruse, W., Smith, M. F, Day, J., Keeling, P. W. N., Scopes, J. W. & Thompson, R. P. H. (1981) Zinc and small babies. Lancet 1: 1135-1136.

Penland, J G. (1991) Cognitive performance effects of low zinc (Zn) intakes in healthy adult men. FASEB J. 5: A938 (abs.).

Peters, D. P. (1979a) Effects of prenatal nutritional deficiency on affiliation and aggression in rats Physiol Behav. 20, 359-362.

Peters, D P. (1979b, Effects of prenatal nutrition on learning and motivation in rats Phys Behav. 22: 1067-1071.

Peters, D. P. (1979c) Effects of prenatal nutritional deficiency on discrimination learning in rats: acquisition and retention. Psychol. Rep. 44: 451-456.

Prasad, A. S (1988) Zinc in growth and development and spectrum of human zinc deficiency. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 7: 377 - 384.

Prasad, A. S., Rabbani, P., Abbasi, A., Bowersox, E. & Fox, M. R. S. (1978) Experimental zinc deficiency in humans. Ann. Int. Med. 89: 483-490

Reeves, P G. & O'Dell, B. L. (1980) Short-term zinc deficiency in the rat and self-selection of dietary protein level. Fed. Proc. 39: 375-383.

Ruz, M., Cavan, K. R., Bettger, W. J. & Gibson, R. S. (1992) Erythrocytes, erythrocyte membranes, neutrophils and platelets as biopsy materials for the assessment of zinc status in humans. Br .J. Nutr. 68: 515-527.

Ruz, M., Cavan, K. R., Bettger, W. J., Thompson, L., Berry, M. & Gibson, R. S. (1991) Development of a dietary model for the study of mild zinc deficiency in humans and evaluation of some biochemical and functional indices of zinc status. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 53: 1295-1303.

Sandstead, H. H., Bolonchuk, W., Inman, L., Johnson, L., Johnson, P., Klevay, L., Lukaski, H., Lykken, B., Mine, D., Mahaldo, J., Tucker, D & Wallwork, .J. (1980) Experimental zinc deficiency in man. Clin Res. 28: 759A (abs.).

Sandstead, H. H., Strobel, D A., Logan, G. M., Jr., Marks, E. O & Jacob, R. A. (1978) Zinc deficiency in pregnant rhesus monkeys: effects on behavior of infants. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 31: 844849.

Sandstead, H., Tucker, D, Wallwork, J., Canfield, W., Klevay, L., Milne, D., Mahaldo, J., Inman, L & Johnson, L. (1981) Neurophysiological effects of mild zinc deficiency in humans. Clin. Res. 29: 754A (abs).

Slomianka, L. (1992), Neurons of origin of zinc-containing pathways and the distribution of zinc-containing boutons in the hippocampal region of the rat. Neurosci. 48: 325-352.

Soumillion, A., Van Damme, J. & DeLey, M (1992) Cloning and specific polymerised-chain-reaction amplication of a third charge-separable human metallothionein isoform. J. Biochem. 209: 9991004.

Strobel, D. A. & Sandstead, H. H. (1984) Social and learning changes following prenatal or postnatal zinc deprivation in rhesus monkeys. In: The Neurobiology of Zinc (Frederickson, C. 1., Howell, G. A. & Kasarskis, E. J., eds.), Part B: Deficiency, Toxicity and Pathology, pp. 121-138. Alan R. Liss, Inc., New York, NY.

Torre, M., Rodriguez, A. R. & Saura-Calixto, F. (1991) Effects of dietary fiber and physic acid on mineral availability. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutri 30: 1-22.

Tucker, D. M. & Sandstead, H. H. (1984) Neuropsychological function in experimental zinc deficiency in humans. In: The Neurobiology of Zinc (Frederickson, C. J., Howell, G. A. & Kasarskis, E. J., eds.), Part B: Deficiency, Toxicity, and Pathology, pp. 139-152 Alan R. Liss, Inc., New York, NY.

Udomkesmalee, E., Dhanamitta, S., Sirisinha, S., Charoenkiatkul, S., Tuntipopipot, S., Banjong, O., Rojroongwasinkul, N., Kramer, T. R. & Smith, J. C., Jr. (1992) Effect of vitamin A and zinc supplementation on the nutriture of children in Northwest Thailand. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 56: 50-57.

Van Wouwe, J. P. (1989) Clinical and laboratory diagnosis of acrodermatitis enteropathica. Eur. J. Pediatr. 149: 2-8.

Wachs, T. D., Bishry, Z., Moussa, W., Yunis, F., McCabe, G., Harrison, G., Sweifi, E., Kirksey, A., Galal, O., Jerome, H. & Shaheen, F. (19951 Nutritional intake and context as predictors of cognition and adaptive behavior of Egyptian school age children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. (in press).

Wallwork, J. C., Fosmire, G. J. a Sandstead, H. H. (1981) Effect of zinc deficiency on appetite and plasma amino acid concentrations in the rat. Br. J. Nutr. 45: 127-136.

Wallwork, J C., Tucker, D. M. & Sandstead, H. H. (1982) Mild Zn deficiency in humans: neuropsychological effects. Fed. Proc. 41: 780 (abs.).

Walravens, P. A., Charkar, A., Mokni, R., Denise, J. & Lemonnier, D. (1992) Zinc supplements in breastfed infants. Lancet 340: 683-685.

Walravens, P. A., Hambidge, K. M. & Koepfer, D. M. (1989) Zinc supplementation in infants with a nutritional pattern of failure to thrive: a double-blind study Pediatr. 83: 532-538.


Contents - Previous - Next