Contents - Previous - Next

This is the old United Nations University website. Visit the new site at http://unu.edu


TABLE 4. Studies of the secondary preventive effects of breastfeeding

Study Study design Sample Exclusion criteria Outcome variables Significant results
Lucas et al., 1989 [34] Prospective, longitudinal, randomized trial compar- ing the effects of donated BBM and PTF on the be- havioural development of pre-tenm infants. Subjects received the treatment until discharge from the hospital or until attaining a weight of 2,000 g Data on behaviour obtained at 9 mo Subjects classified into 2 groups according to mother's decision to breastfeed or not to breastfeed Within each group, subjects randomly assigned to receive BBM or PTF via nasogastric tube while in hospital Treatment was given as a supplement to mother's milk to infants whose mothers chose to breast feed, and as sole diet to in- fants whose mothers chose not to breastfeed 502 pre-term infants born in 3 hospitals in England and admitted to special care unit No. of subjects included in final analysis: Breastfed group: mother's milk + BBM (n = 170); mother's milk + PTF (n = 173) Not breastfed group: BBM only (n = 83); PTF only (n = 76) Birthweight >1,850 g Major congenital anomalies Scores on developmental screening inventory of Knobloch et al. at 9 mot Five areas of behaviour assessed: adaptive be- haviour, gross motor fine motor, language and personal-social Overall DQ obtained by averaging scores on all scales Comparisons for all subjects receiv ing BBM vs subjects receiving PTF at 9 mo regardless of whether or not they received mother's milk:
  BBM PTF
overall DQ 97.9 100.4*
adaptive 101.3 104.0*
gross motor 99.9 102.3
fine motor 96.6 99.2**
language 93.3 95.6**
personal-social 98.0 100.6**
*p < .05; **p < .025
Comparisons for subjects receiving treatment as supplement to mother's milk (breasfed group):
  BBM PTF
overall DQ 98.2 101.2*
adaptive 102.1 105.0**
gross motor 100.4 103.7
fine motor 97.2 100.0
language 93.1 95.8**
personal-social 97.7 101.5*
*p < .05; **p < .025
Comparisons for subjects receiving treatments as sole diets (not breasfed group) were not significant
Lucas et al., 1994 [35] Prospective, longitudinal, randomized trial compar- ing the effects of donated BBM and PTF on the cog- nitive and motor develop- ment of pre-tenm infants Data on cognitive and motor performance gathered at 10 mo Subjects classified mto 2 groups according to mother's decision to breastfeed or not to breastfeed Within each group, subjects randomly assigned to receive BBM or PTF via nasogastric tube while in hospital Treatment was given as a supplement to mother's milk to infants whose mothers chose to breast feed, and as sole diet to infants whose mothers chose not to breastfeed 502 pre-term infants born in 3 hospitals in England and admitted to special care unit No. of subjects included in final analysis: Breastfed group: mother's milk + BBM (n = 134); mother's milk + PTF (n = 139) Not breastfed group: BBM only (n = 62); PTF only (n = 52) Birthweight >1,850 g Major congenital anomalies Bayley MDI and PDI scores at 18 mo No significant effects of treatment type (BBM or PTF) or of pres ence or absence of breastfeeding on MDI or PDI scores
Lucas et al., 1992 [36] 8-yr follow-up of cognitive development of pre-term infants participating in 2 prospective randomized clinical triais Trial 1 compared the effects of regular formula and PTF on cognitive and motor performance of pre-tenm infants at 10 mo Tnal 2 compared the effects of BBM and PTF on cog- nitive and motor perfor mance of pre-tenm infants at 9 and 18 mo 300 children 7.5-8 yr old bonn pre-tenm and admitted to special care unit at 5 hospitals in England Birthweight >1,850 g Major congenital anomalies Scores on abbreviated WISC-R Test at 7.5-8 yr . Mean score advantages for breasfed vs not breastfed children on various WISC-R scales:
Scale Advantage
verbal +10.1
performance +10.1
overall IQ +10.2
p < .001
Analyses for randomization trial outcomes (e g., regular formula reported
Lucas et al., 1992 [36] Treatment was given as a supplement to mother's milk to infants whose mothers chose to breast feed, and as sole diet to in fants whose mothers chose not to breastfeed Subjects classified in 2 groups according to whether or not they were breastfed: breastfed + treatment (n = 210); not breastfed (treatment only) (n = 90) Data on cognitive perfor mance obtained at 7.5-8 yr        
Lanting et al., 1994 [38] Retrospective correlational study examining the rela- tion between infant-feed- ing method and develop- ment of neurologic disorders in childhood Subjects classified soon after birth as having normal, slightly abnormal, or frankly abnormal neuro- logic status on the basis of a standardized neurologic examination Subjects selected for follow up given a standardized neurologic examination at 9 yr Subjects classified according to infant-feeding practices on the basis of a question naire completed by parents: formula-fed (n = 358); breastfed with formula supplement at least 3 wk (n = 33); exclusively breastfed at least 3 wk (n = 135) Selected from cohort of 3,162 infants bonn 1975-78 in a hospital in the Netherlands Feeding data in- complete Mother could not recall feeding practices Pre-term infants Infant admitted to special care ward Performance on neuro- logic examination at 9 yr Neurologic status classi- fied as: normal, minor dysfunction-grade I (fewer symptoms), minor dysfunction- grade 2 (more symp- toms), or abnormal For analysis, subjects breastfed with formula supplement at least 3 wk were combined with subjects exclusively breastfed at least 3 wk For all subjects breastfed at least 3 wk, regardless of neurologic classification at birth, scores on the 9-yr neurologic examination and the likelihood of being classified as normal at 9 yr were higher
Menkes, 1977 [39] Retrospective, cross-sec- tional, correlational study examining the relation between infant-feeding method and development of learning disabilities in childhood Subjects were classified as those with learning dis- orders and a control group of children with other neurologic disorders Breastfed children defined as those receiving breastmilk for at least 4 wk Learning disorder sub- jects were 29 white, middle-class chil- dren born 1959-70 and referred to author for neuro- logic assessment and treatment Control subjects were patients of the author who pre sensed with a variety of other neurologic disorders Subjects matched by sex and age (n = 53) Pre-term infants (no definition provided) Major congenital malformations Infants whose feeding methods were determined by circumstances Subjects in the learning disorder and control groups were classified as breastfed or boale- fed 13.8% of children with learning disorders and 47.2% of control children were breastfed (P = .004)

A natural conclusion from this study is that at nine months of age, pre-term babies who were fed a PTF with the specifications listed had a developmental advantage over those fed BBM. The authors note that part of the developmental importance of this finding is that the screening inventory includes many of the items of the Gesell Schedule, which is a good predictor of later intelligence. However, in contrast to this claim, a vast body of literature on infant development shows that the predictive validity of developmental scales administered during the first 12 months of life is not different from zero [11]. In our view, the reported differences in the performance of preterm infants fed PTF and BBM are anecdotally interesting but of limited developmental significance over the long term.

In a follow-up at 18 months of age, Lucas et al. [35] assessed 387 of the infants in the original sample with the Bayley MDI and PDI scales. In contrast to the nine-month follow-up, no advantage was found for subjects receiving PTF; infants fed with BBM performed about as well as subjects receiving PTF on both the MDI and PDI scales. Additionally, no significant differences were found for MDI or PDI scores when the subjects were grouped according to whether or not they received mother's milk as a supplement. The authors suggest that the failure to find results in the same direction as the nine-month follow-up may be due to the subjects' recovery from earlier nutritional deficiencies.

In a 1992 article, Lucas et al. [36] reported the effects of breastmilk fed to pre-term babies on intelligence quotients obtained seven to eight years later (table 4). In this second study, the subjects were admitted to the special-care baby units in Cambridge, Ipswich, Kings Lynn, Norwich, and Sheffield between January 1982 and March 1985. Two of these care units (Norwich and Sheffield) were not included in the study published in 1989. Also, in the first study the subjects were selected up to 1984, whereas in this second study the recruitment extended to March 1985. The authors do not specify the degree of overlap between the subjects in the 1994 study [35] and those in the present study. However, they do suggest that both fall out from the same clinical trial and follow the same design. One feature of the 1992 publication is that it does not include data on the subjects who received only BBM.

At 7 1/2 to 8 years of age, the subjects who had received milk expressed from their own mothers (group 2) had higher IQs than the children whose mothers chose not to provide breastmilk for them (group 1). In particular, the differences were observed in the Verbal Performance and Overall Scale (group 1, 92.8; group 2, 103) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. According to an analysis within group 2 of the subjects who received breastmilk from their mothers in comparison with those who did not, the overall IQ of this last subgroup (94.8) was similar to that of group 1 (92.8), and both groups had significantly lower IQs than children whose mothers were successful in expressing milk (103.7). A final analysis restricted to the children of the successful mothers showed a dose-response relationship between mother's milk and subsequent IQ.

In conclusion, feeding maternal milk to pre-term babies with very low birthweights improved their middle-childhood IQs, as compared with IQs of preterm babies who did not receive maternal milk (and who received PTF, mature, pasteurized donor drip breastmilk, or term formula). The authors concluded that the data supported the causal hypothesis that breastmilk promotes the neural development of pre-term babies. They found additional support for their hypothesis in the existing information that human milk contains various factors, such as long chain lipids, that might affect nervous system development. This impressive set of findings requires confirmation by other laboratories and a control for the extent to which the mothers who succeeded in expressing milk did or did not differ in their childrearing practices from the remaining mothers.

Lanting et al. [38] studied the relation between feeding method and necrologic functioning at nine years of age (table 4). Three groups of children were formed on the basis of a neonatal neurologic examination. One group (n = 160) was defined as neurologically abnormal (e.g., hemisyndrome, hypotonia, hypertonia) at birth; a second group consisted of a random sample of children (n = 322) with minor abnormalities (e.g., mild hypotonia); and a random sample of normal children formed a third group (n = 322). Nine years after birth, a standard neurologic examination was given to the children in the three groups and they were again reclassified as normal, having minor neurologic dysfunction (two categories), or abnormal. In addition, information was obtained at this new examination on early feeding practices, and three feeding groups were formed: exclusively formula-fed, breastfed plus formula-fed within the first three weeks of birth, or exclusively breastfed for at least three weeks.

Across groups, at nine years of age the children who had been exclusively breastfed were neurologically better off than the formula-fed children. Moreover, among the children classified with minor neurologic dysfunction during the neonatal period, those who had been breastfed were less likely to have minor neurologic dysfunction. In particular, the frequency of minor neurologic abnormalities was twice as high for formula-fed children as for those who were breastfed. The authors suggested that the type of feeding during the first weeks of life plays a role in later neurobehavioural development.

Of interest here is a retrospective study published in 1977 which showed that early feeding history predicted learning disorders [39] (table 4). The frequency of breastfeeding was compared in two groups of children: 29 children referred to a paediatric neurologist because of learning disorders and 53 children seen in the same office because of neurologic conditions other than learning disorders. In the latter group, 47.2% of the children had been breastfed, compared with only 13.8% in the former group. The intent of this comparative study was to test the hypothesis that the high protein content (1.5 to 3.3 g/100 ml) of the formula contributed to the evolution of learning disorders.

Breastmilk as risk factor (table 5)

Two studies are reviewed here that address the question of whether breastmilk operates as a risk factor by the transfer of toxic substances from the mother to the infant [40, 41]. Because of their correlational nature, these studies have limitations in design similar to those discussed above regarding the direct effects of breastmilk. There is no way of discriminating between group differences due to the independent variable and those due to confounders, nor can the effects associated with the prenatal and postnatal periods be separated. A particular concern is whether the use of a toxic substance is associated with caretaking behaviours that hinder rather than foster development.

The first study was a prospective, longitudinal study that tested the effects of alcohol consumption during the lactation period on mental and motor development at 12 months of age [40]. The subjects were 400 middle-class infants. The independent variable was represented by a score that indicated the infant's exposure to alcohol through breastmilk. Maternal reports of daily alcohol consumption were obtained along with reports of the number of breastfeeding days per month. These scores were then related to the Bayley PDI and MDI scores at 12 months of age. A statistically significant linear trend was observed between alcohol consumption and PDI. A comparison of the end points of this continuum shows a difference of 19 PDI points (>1 standard deviation) between infants of mothers who consumed no alcohol and those with the highest consumption.

The second study, which was also longitudinal and correlational, examined the potential effects of the use of marijuana during lactation on the mental and motor development of the offspring [41]. Of importance here is that marijuana appears in the milk of lactating women who use the drug [42]. The criterion for inclusion in the study was breastfeeding for at least two weeks. As in other studies, the Bayley MDI and PDI scores were obtained from all infants at 12 months of age. Again, although there were no differences between children of users and non-users of marijuana in the MDI, there was a clear trend in the expected direction for the PDI. The mean PDI of the infants of mothers with 0 days of exposure was 102, whereas that of the infants with 15 to 30 days of exposure was 90.

As noted, inherent limitations of correlational studies prevent conclusive inferences. Moreover, even under controlled conditions, data from two studies alone are generally insufficient to guide policy. Therefore, definitive recommendations regarding breastfeeding for mothers who consume alcohol and marijuana are unwarranted. However, the findings are suggestive and indicate a need for further information.

General discussion

Breastfed children have a modest advantage over artificially fed children in developmental scales, IQ tests, and tests of particular cognitive processes. Although no definitive conclusions on causality are warranted, the consistency of the findings across studies carried out in different populations and social contexts is quite remarkable. These suggestive findings point to the need for experimental studies leading to conclusive statements.

In addition to limitations of design, the studies have had a restricted focal concern, first, on main effects, and second, on products of development such as IQ or psychomotor indexes. Possible differential effects of breastfeeding as a function of contextual (e.g., social and economic) factors and organismic status have been disregarded. There is also suggestive evidence that the duration of breastfeeding may have an effect.

Rather than focusing on developmental outcomes, studies should also examine the contributions of breastfeeding to the processes of development. In this line of reasoning, we believe that a particular area of enormous potential is the assessment of the role of breastfeeding in the formation of secure attachment in early childhood. Besides their universality, breastfeeding and attachment have had significant evolutionary value, and their interconnectedness could well have served human adaptation.

TABLE 5. Studies of breastmilk as a risk factor

Study Study design Sample Exclusion criteria Outcome variables Significant results
Little et al. 1989 [40] Prospective, longitudinal, correlational study examin- ing the effects of maternal alcohol use during lactation on infant mental and motor development Cognitive and motor develop- ment measured at 12 mo Maternal alcohol use measured by questionnaire during mo 1 and 3 Breastfed children defined as those receiving the majority of intake from breastmilk (<16 oz/day of non-human milk or supplement) 400 infants, mostly white and middle-class, born to mothers belonging to a health maintenance organization in Seattle, Washington, USA No prenatal care before 6 mo gestation Bayley MDI and PDI scores at 12 mo Independent variable: Infant AA score, used to indicate infant's ex- posure to alcohol via breastmilk, estimated from mother's reports of daily alcohol con- gumption and days of breastfeeding per month during mo 1 and 3 Infant AA score not related to MDI scores at 12 mo Significant linear trend found for effects of infant AA score on PDI score at 12 mo (p < .006)
AA score n PDI
0 111 104
>0-0.4 188 103
0.5-0.9 71 99
1-1.4 25 98
>=1.5 4 85
Astley and Little, 1990 [41] Longitudinal, correlational study examining the relation between maternal marijuana use during lactation and cognitive and motor devel- opment of infants Cognitive and motor perfor- mance measured at 12 mo breastfed children defined as those receiving the majonty of intake from breastmilk (<16 oz/day of non-human milk or supplement) for at least 2 wk 323 infants, mostly white and middle-class, born to mothers belonging to a health maintenance organization in Seattle, Washington, USA Infants not breastfed at least 2 wk Infants who did not have 12-mo assessments Bayley MDI and PDI scores at 12 mo Independent variable: Infant's exposure to matemal marijuana use during lactation esti- mated from mother's reports of daily mari- juana use and days of breastfeedmg per month during mo 1 and 3 . No significant effect of exposure to marijuana on MDI scores at 12 mo Mean PDI scores at 12 mo according to days of exposure to maternal marijuana use during Ist month postpartum:
Days exposure n PDI
0 81 102
1-14 38 106
15-30 17 90
Mean difference between groups with 0 and 1-14 days exposure and group with 15-30 days exposure is significant (p < .005).

The suggestive evidence that breastmilk contributes to the healthy mental development of premature infants is impressive. Moreover, the use of breastmilk as an experimental variable provides guidelines for future research with at-risk and normal newborns. Even though the data refer to premature infants with very low birthweight, these findings contradict much of the contemporary theorizing and research data on the causes and risk factors of development. As noted above, we accept that the causes of development are multiple and interactive, and that individuals reach the same end point by different developmental trajectories [43, 44]. It is indeed curious that differences in feeding for 27 days could orient development along very different courses. There is a great need to replicate these findings, which are of major importance to clinical paediatrics.

The external validity of these data is limited, and their relevance for populations where malnutrition is endemic is questionable. The high prevalence of low birthweight in third world countries is attributed primarily to the number of small-for-date newborns [45, 46]. Pre-term birth is a condition that is more prevalent in industrialized societies. The pathophysiology of prematurity is different from that of intrauterine growth retardation.

A natural extension of the data on pre-term babies is the idea that breastmilk operates as a secondary preventive factor. Pre-term babies will benefit from breastmilk because it is rich in LC-PUFA, particularly docosahexaenoic acid, an essential component in the development of the visual system and cerebrum. The correlational work by Lanting et al. [38] and the controlled study by Agostoni et al. [32] support this notion.

The nutritional and immunologic value of human milk is well established, and the suggestive evidence of beneficial behavioural consequences fits with our understanding of the physiological processes involved in lactation. Societal changes, economic pressures, and technological advancements, however, could impose limitations in some instances on the value of human milk. It is plausible that breastmilk transfers drugs and chemicals used by the mother to the nursing infant, which could have adverse developmental consequences. The data on alcohol and marijuana which we reviewed are insufficient to draw any conclusive inferences, but they certainly post a sign for caution.

 

References

1. Birch E, Birch D, Hoffman D, Hale L, Everett M, Uany R. Breast-feeding and optimal visual development. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 1993;30:33-8.

2. Kohn G. Sawatzki G. van Biervliet JP, Rosseneu J. Diet and the essential fatty acid status of term infants. Acta Paediatr Suppl 1994;402:69-74.

3. Neuringer M. Cerebral cortex docosahexaenoic acid is lower in formula-fed than in breast-fed infants. Nutr Rev 1993;51:23841.

4. Kaye K. Toward the origins of dialogue. In: Schaffer HR, ed. Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press, 1977:89-118.

5. Morrow-Tlucak M, Haude RH, Ernhart CB. Breastfeeding and cognitive development in the first 2 years of life. Soc Sci Med 1988;26:635-8.

6. Temboury MC, Otero A, Polanco 1, Arribás E. Influence of breast-feeding on the infant's intellectual development. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1994;18:326.

7. Rogan WJ, Gladen BC. Breast-feeding and cognitive development. Early Hum Dev 1993;31:181-93.

8. Young HB, Buckley AK, Hamza B. Mandarano C. Milk and lactation: some social and developmental correlates among 1,000 infants. Pediatrics 1982;69:16975.

9. Florey CD, Leech AM, Blackhall A. Infant feeding and mental and motor development at 18 months of age in first born singletons. Int J Epidemiol 1995;24:S21-6.

10. Pollitt E, Oh S-Y. Early supplementary feeding, child development and health policy. Food Nutr Bull 199394;15(3):208-14.

11. Colombo J. Infant cognition: predicting later intellectual functioning. Newbury Park, Calif, USA: Sage Publications, 1993.

12. Hoefer C, Hardy MC. Later development of breast-fed and artificially fed infants. JAMA 1929;92:615-9.

13. Fergusson DM, Beautrais AL, Silva PA. Breast-feeding and cognitive development in the first seven years of life. Soc Sci Med 1982;16:1705-8.

14. Rodgers B. Feeding in infancy and later ability and attainment: a longitudinal study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1978;20:421-6.

15. Sameroff AJ, Chandler MJ. Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In: Horowitz FD, ed. Reviews of child development research. Chicago, I11, USA: University of Chicago Press, 1975:187-244.

16. Horowitz FD. Using developmental theory to guide the search for the effects of biological risk factors on the development of children. Am J Clin Nutr Suppl 1989; 50:589-97.

17. Sameroff AJ, McDonough SC. The role of motor activity in human cognitive and social development. In: Pollitt E, Amante P, eds. Energy intake and activity (Current Topics in Nutrition and Disease 11). New York: Alan R. Liss, 1984:33153.

18. Gorman KS, Pollitt E. Does schooling buffer the effects of early risk? Child Dev 1996;67:314-26.

19. Dick-Read G. Childbirth without fear: the principles and practice of natural childbirth. 2nd ed. New York: Harper, 1959.

20. Eyer D. Mother-infant bonding: a scientific fiction. New Haven, Conn, USA: Yale University Press, 1992.

21. Pollitt E. Iron deficiency and cognitive function. Annu Rev Nutr 1993:521-37.

22. Idjradinata P. Pollitt E. Reversal of developmental delays in iron-deficient anaemic infants treated with iron. Lancet 1993;341:14.

23. Lonnerdal B. Iron and breast-milk. In: Stekel A, ed. Iron nutrition in infancy and childhood (Nestle Nutrition Workshop Series 4). New York: Raven Press, 1984:95-114.

24. Moffatt MEK, Longstaffe S. Beant l, Dureski C. Prevention of iron deficiency and psychomotor decline in high-risk infants through use of iron-fortified infant formula: a randomized clinical trial. J Pediatr 1994;125: 188-534.

25. Siimes MA. Iron nutrition in low-birth-weight infants. In: Stekel A, ed. Iron nutrition in infancy and childhood (Nestle Nutrition Workshop Series 4). New York: Raven Press, 1984:75-90.

26. Cohen RJ, Landa Rivera L, Rivera A, Lonnerdal B. Brown KH, Dewey K. Anemia among breast-fed infants at 6 months of age in Honduras. XV International Congress of Nutrition in Australia: Final Program and Abstracts. Adelaide, Australia: 1993;2:844.

27. Crawford MA, Hassam AG, Williams G. Essential fatty acids and fetal brain growth. Lancet 1976;1:452-53

28. Yamamoto N. Saitoh M, Moriuchi A, Okuyama H. Effect of dietary alfa-linolenate/linoleate balance on brain lipid composition and learning ability of rats. J Lipid Res 1987;28:144 51.

29. Uany R. Andraca I. Human milk and breast feeding for optimal mental development. J Nutr 1995;125:2278S-80S.

30. Kotlezko B. Bremer HD. Fat content and fatty acid composition of infant formulae. Acta Paediatr Scand 1989;78:513-21.

31. Carlson SE. Dietary fatty acids in relation to neural development in infants. In: Galli C, Simopolous AP, eds. Dietary w3 and w6 fatty acids: biological effects of nutritional essentiality. New York: Plenum Press, 1989: 113546.

32. Agostoni C, Trojan S. Bellu R. Riva E, Giovannini M. Neurodevelopmental quotient of healthy term infants at 4 months and feeding practice: the role of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. Pediatr Res 1995;38: 262-6.

33. Uauy R. Birch D, Birch E, Tyson J. Hoffman D. Effect of dietary omega-3 fatty acids on retinae function of very low-birth weight neonates. Pediatr Res 1990;28: 485-92.

34. Lucas A, Morley R. Cole TJ, Gore SM, Davis JA, Bamford MFM, Dossetor JFB. Early diet in preterm babies and developmental status in infancy. Arch Dis Child 1989;64:1578.

35. Lucas A, Morley R. Cole TJ, Gore SM. A randomised multicentre study of human milk versus formula and later development in preterm infants. Arch Dis Child 1994;70:F1416.

36. Lucas A, Morley R. Cole TJ, Lister G. Leeson-Payne C. Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet 1992;339:261-4.

37. Knobloch KH, Pasamanick B. Sherard ES. A developmental screening inventory for infants. Pediatrics 1966; 38:1095-108.

38. Lanting CI, Fidler V, Huisman M, Touwen BCL, Boersma ER. Neurological differences between 9-year-old children fed breast-milk or formula-milk as babies. Lancet 1994;344:131922.

39. Menkes JH. Early feeding history of children with learning disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol 1977;19: 169-71.

40. Little RE, Anderson KW, Ervin, CH, Worthington-Roberts B. Clarren SK. Maternal alcohol use during breast-feeding and infant mental and motor development at one year. N Engl J Med 1989;321:425-30.

41. Astley SJ, Little RE. Maternal marijuana use during lactation and infant development at one year. Neurotoxicol Teratol 1990;12:161-8.

42. Subcommittee on Nutrition During Lactation. Infant outcomes. In: Nutrition during lactation. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1991;7:153-96.

43. Futterweit LR, Ruff HA. Principles of development: implications for early intervention. J Appl Dev Psych 1993;14:153-73.

44. Turkewitz G. Devenny DA. Timing and the shape of development. In: Turkewitz G. Devenny DA, eds. Developmental time and timing. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1993:1-11.

45. Villar J. Belizán JM. The timing factor in the pathophysiology of the intrauterine growth retardation syndrome. Obstet Gynecol Surv 1982;37:499.

46. Belizán JM, Villar J. El crecimiento fetal y su repercusión sobre el desarrollo del nino. In: Cusminsky M , More no EM , Suárez-Oecda EN, eds. Crecimiento y desarrollo. Washington, DC: Organización Panamericana de la Salud: Publicación Científica, 1988;10219.


Contents - Previous - Next