This is the old United Nations University website. Visit the new site at http://unu.edu
Development of indicators: A note of caution
Various quantitative methods have been developed and employed successfully to assess relevant features of psychosocial care. These include questionnaires or interviews asking the parents or caregivers to describe their care practices and/or their children's typical experiences; "qualitative" rating scales with behaviourally defined scale points to assess broadly stated characteristics of care, following observations in naturalistic or experimental settings; systematic observations of the caregiver and the child in the natural setting, focusing on particular behaviours whose frequencies are reflected in summary scores; and detailed observational coding of ongoing sequences of caregiver-child interaction, aimed at characterizing relevant features of the dyadic relationship.
Many of these strategies and techniques for assessing quality of care in the various research studies require extensive observations and technical methodologies. For the most part, therefore, they are not feasible for practical use in the field as screening procedures. However, some of these approaches to the measurement of care have been, or may be, adapted for more practical field use in assessing breakdowns in, or threats to, psychosocial care. The relevance of these measures in different cultures should always be of concern to investigators. It is important in all such approaches to develop indicators that have meaning and validity across a variety of cultures, although this is a daunting task. A behaviour that may appear to be maladaptive to an outside observer may be the norm within a particular culture. Therefore, it is essential to approach indicators with a healthy skepticism, and to attempt to use a within-culture yardstick-to compare a caregiver with others within her or his own cultural group.
The Caring Analysis Tool
Before planning or developing an intervention, it is essential to assess the current status of psychosocial care. If the time of the caregiver is extremely limited, the approach may require identifying alternatives. If food or health is severely limited, care alone is unlikely to make a difference. A possible analytic strategy for dealing with this general problem is outlined in the Caring Analysis Tool shown in table 2 [39]. This technique incorporates a series of questions that can be adapted by local programme planners in thinking through the implications of women and children's care for policies, strategies, and programmes. The questions are shown in table 2 in an expanded form. Brief forms for regular use and for quick scanning are also included.
The Caring Analysis Tool is intended to be used in addition to other strategies to increase programme effectiveness. Successful nutrition programmes are flexible, give priority to high-risk environments, use a multifaceted and integrated approach, and are community based and participatory [105].
Summary
There is considerable evidence that poor psychosocial care is associated with unfavourable cognitive development and poorer nutrition and growth outcomes in young children in both industrialized and developing countries. Mothers or other primary caregivers of malnourished children have been reported to be less sensitive to the child's needs, less involved and emotionally responsive, and less engaged in reciprocal interaction with their children than mothers or other primary caregivers of adequately or well-nourished children. They have also been described as somewhat passive, lacking in selfesteem, and socially isolated. Although these associations seem somewhat more marked and consistent in the case of children experiencing clinical or severe malnutrition, they have been observed in children with chronic undernutrition as well.
How can these reported associations best be interpreted? It is important not to view the relatively unfavourable childcare and maternal characteristics associated with malnutrition as being personal flaws or deficiencies of the mother or other primary caregiver. One needs to be aware of the major economic, social, and resource constraints that exert their pressures on poor families and make it difficult for them to provide high-quality child care. Some of the factors that influence the availability of psychosocial care are the caregivers' beliefs in their self efficacy or developmental norms; the caregivers' stress, anxiety, or depression; the caregivers' income control and autonomy; the presence of social support from family and community; and the availability of alternative child care.
With regard to the important question of directions of influence, it is apparent that the features of psychosocial care just summarized may be viewed either as responses to the phenotypic or behavioural characteristics of a sickly or malnourished child, or as an indicator of poor quality of overall child care contributing to the child's malnutrition, or both. It is probably best to view these associations as representing bidirectional or transactional pathways of influence, which may be self-sustaining in the absence of some form of preventive intervention. Perhaps enhancement of the young child's motor, mental, and social development through improved psychosocial care makes the child a more efficient "elicitor" of good nutritional care from parent or caregiver, thus increasing the likelihood of improved nutritional outcomes. Alternatively, such a psychosocial intervention may lead to a general improvement in the quality of nutritional as well as psychosocial care provided by the mother or primary caregiver. Likewise, findings suggest that enhancing the very young child's motor and behavioural competencies through improved health and nutrition may, in turn, render the child a more effective elicitor of good nutritional care.
Additional research is needed to clarify the nature of these bidirectional and transactional pathways of influence. There is a need to move beyond correlational studies to experimental designs. In the meantime, it is very apparent that intervention or preventive educational programmes to improve nutrition should include systematic efforts to support positive caregiver-child interactions and to optimize the quality of overall care infants and young children receive, in both the psychosocial and the nutritional, health, and physical care arenas. Such integrative efforts are needed not only to reduce the risk of early malnutrition's occurring in the first place, but also to promote the growth and behavioural development of children whose nutrition status may already have been compromised.
TABLE 2. Caring analysis tool
Part I. Assessment of existing situation
Describe the current caregiving situation.
1. What are the relative roles of inadequate household food security, lack of health services or an unhealthy environment, or lack of care in generating malnutrition in the current situation?
2. What are the most common care and support environments in which caregiving occurs? Who takes care of the children, where are they cared for, and who is supporting the family? Are care and support resources adequate? Are there untapped care and support resources?
3. In what age or developmental period (prenatal, newborn, infant, one- to two-year-old, pre-schooler) are the children who are the most in need?
4. Are there good caregiving beliefs or behaviours already in existence that could be built upon?
5. Are there basic societal reasons for the lack of availability of care?
Part II. Programme actions: effects of care on nutrition
As the programme develops, what kinds of changes in caregiving behaviours, beliefs, or knowledge will result?
1. What are the behaviours and the cultural beliefs underlying these behaviours that the programme is encouraging, and who is likely to perform these behaviours?
2. What other activities or belief systems will the primary caregiver forego in order to perform these new caring behaviours?
3. Did the programme consider the influence of psychological and social factors on the mother or child? Was there a concern for these factors in prenatal care, birth procedures, or breastfeeding promotion? Was there a concern for the social and emotional aspects of the weaning and feeding process?
Part III. Programme actions: effect of the child on care
Did the programme consider ways in which characteristics of the child might influence the kinds of caregiving received? Are there parental beliefs that might mean that certain kinds of children are more likely to benefit from the programme than others?
1. Will girl children be affected more or less than boy children?
2. Will the child's anorexia or appetite, energy level, or health or nutrition status influence his or her programme participation?
3. Are there other conditions, such as physical handicaps, that might influence programme participation?
4. Does the programme consider developmental changes in the child's needs for care? Is the programme addressing the appropriate age group?
Part IV. Programme actions: constraints to care
Describe the availability of care at either the household or the community level, and identify possible constraints to that care. Are any of the following factors constraints to care? If so, how can the constraint be ameliorated?
1. Physical ill health of the mother or other caregiver.
2. Lack of self-confidence, depression, or stress.
3. Beliefs about child-rearing and feeding, about who should control the amount of food young children eat, and lack of education or information about child-rearing practices that would achieve parents' goals for children.
4. Excessive workload of mother or other caregiver to achieve household food security; excessive workload for home production and child care or inequitable division of labour; lack of responsibility of other family members (e.g., father).
5. Lack of social support: emotional support, instrumental support (assistance), or informational support from family, community, or services.
6. Absence of resources or lack of control of resources by caregiver.
Part V. Programme actions: basic causes of constraints to care
Has the programme attempted to address any of the basic societal causes of constraints to care observed initially?
1. Technical or material conditions of production.
2. Social conditions of production.
3. Political factors.
4. Ideological or cultural factors.
Part Vl. Evaluation
What indicators of changes in caring behaviours could be used to assess the effectiveness of the programme? Are there measures to assess changes in the caregiver's behaviour, in the child's behaviour, and in developmental outcomes for children?
References
1. Gottfried AK, ed. Home environment and early cognitive development: longitudinal research. New York: Academic Press, 1984.
2. Wachs TD, Gruen GE. Early experience and human development. New York: Plenum Press, 1982.
3. Engle PL. An interactive model of child care and nutrition with implications for programs. UNICEF Series, Cornell Food and Nutrition Policy Program. Ithaca, NY, USA: Cornell University Press, 1992.
4. LeVine RA. Child rearing as cultural adaptation. In: Leiderman P. Tulkin SR, Rosenfeld A, eds. Culture and infancy: variations in the human experience. New York: Academic Press, 1977:15-28.
5. Zeitlin M, Ghassemi H. Mansour M. Positive deviance in child nutrition. Tokyo: United Nations University Press, 1990.
6. Pollitt E, Gorman K, Engle P. Martorell R. Rivera J. Early supplementary feeding and cognition: effects over two decades. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1993;58:235.
7. Chen LC, Hug E, d'Souza S. Sex bias in the family allocation of food and health care in rural Bangladesh. Pop Dev Rev 1981;7:55-70.
8. Ravindran S. Health implications of sex discrimination in childhood: a review paper as an annotated bibliography. Geneva: WHO, 1986.
9. Caldwell B. Bradley R. Home observation for measurement of the environment. Little Rock, Ark, USA: University of Arkansas Press, 1984.
10. Bradley RH, Caldwell BM, Rock SL, Barnard KE, Gray C, Hammond MA, Mitchell S. Siegel L, Ramey CT, Gotfried AW, Johnson DL. Home environment and cognitive development in the first three years of life: a collaborative study involving six sites and three ethnic groups in North America. Dev Psych 1989;25: 217-35.
11. Bee HL, Barnard KE, Eyres SJ, Gray CA, Hammond MA, Spietz AL, Snyder C, Clark B. Prediction of IQ and language skills from perinatal status, child performance, family characteristics, and mother-infant interaction. Child Dev 1982; 53:1134-56.
12. Bakeman R. Brown JV. Early interaction: consequences for social and mental development at three years. Child Dev 1980;51:437-47.
13. Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM. Mother-infant interaction and the development of competence. In: Connolly K, Bruner J. eds. The growth of competence. New York: Academic Press, 1974:97-118.
14. Beckwith L, Cohen S. Home environment and cognitive competence. In: Gottfried AW, ed. Home environment and early cognitive development. New York: Academic Press, 1984:235-71.
15. Olson SL, Bayales I, Bates JE. Mother-child interactions and children's speech progress: a longitudinal study of the first two years. Merril-Palmer Q 1986;32: 1-20.
16. Wachs TD. The nature of nurture. Newbury Park, Calif, USA: Sage, 1992.
17. Williams JR, Scott RB. Growth and development of Negro infants: IV. Motor development and its relationship to child rearing practices in two groups of Negro infants. Child Dev 1953;24:103-21.
18. Campbell RA, Ramey CT. Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: a follow-up study of children from low income families. Child Dev 1994:65:684-98.
19. Ainsworth MDS. Infant-mother attachment. Am Psych 1979;34:932-7.
20. Isabella RA, Belsky J. Origins of attachment: maternal interactive behavior across the first year. Child Dev 1993:64:605-21.
21. Pederson DR, Moran G. Sitko C, Campbell K, Ghesquire K, Acton H. Maternal sensitivity and the security of infant-mother attachment. Child Dev 1990; 61:1974-83.
22. DeLicardie ET, Cravioto J. Behavioral responsiveness of survivors of clinically severe malnutrition to cognitive demands. In: Cravioto J. et al., eds. Early malnutrition and mental development. Upsala, Sweden: Almquist and Wiksell, 1974:134-53.
23. Agarwal DK, Awasthy A, Upadhyay SK, Singh P. Kumar J. Agarwa KN. Growth, behavior, development, and intelligence in rural children between 1-3 years of life. Indian Pediatr 1992;29:467-80.
24. Church AT, Katigbak MS. Home environment, nutritional status, and maternal intelligence as determinants of intellectual development in rural Philippine preschool children. Intelligence 1991;15:49-78.
25. Sigman M, Neumann C, Carter E, Cattle DJ, D'Souza S. Bwibo N. Home interactions and the development of Embu toddlers in Kenya. Child Dev 1988;59:125161.
26. Wachs TD, Bishray Z. Sobhy A, McCabe G. Galal O. Shaheen F. Relation of rearing environment to adaptive behavior of Egyptian toddlers. Child Dev 1993;64:586-604.
27. Grantham-McGregor S, Powell CA, Walker SP, Himes JH. Nutritional supplementation, psychosocial stimulation, and mental development of stunted children: the Jamaica study. Lancet 1991;338:1-5.
28. Grantham-McGregor S, Schofield W, Powell C. Development of severely malnourished children who received psychosocial stimulation: six-year follow-up. Pediatrics 1987;79:247-54.
29. Grantham-McGregor S, Powell C, Walker S, Chang S, Fletcher P. The long-term follow-up of severely malnourished children who participated in an intervention program. Child Dev 1994;65:428-39.
30. Ricciuti HN. Adverse environmental and nutritional influences on mental development: a perspective. J Am Diet Assoc 1981;79:115-20.
31. Ricciuti HN, Dorman R. Interaction of multiple factors contributing to high-risk parenting. In: Hoekelman RA, ed. Minimizing high risk parenting. Media, Pa, USA: Harwal, 1983:187-210.
32. Cravioto J, DeLicardie ER. Microenvironmental factors in severe protein-calorie malnutrition. In: Scrimshaw NS, Behar M, eds. Nutrition and agricultural development: significance and potential for the tropics. New York: Plenum Press, 1976:25-35.
33. Salcedo-Rocha AL, Prado-Aguilar CA. Factors associated with acute malnutrition in migrating preschool children from the Jalisco sugar cane area of Mexico. Bol Med Hosp Inf Mex 1991;48:131-9.
34. Khin-Maung U. Myo K, Nyunt NW, Nyi WH, Their TM, Butler T. Risk factors for the development of persistent diarrhea and malnutrition in Burmese children. Int J Epidemiol 1992;21: 1021-9.
35. Zeitlin MF. Child care and nutrition: the findings from positive deviance research. Final report to UNICEF. New York: UNICEF, 1993.
36. Shekar M, Habicht JP, Latham MC. Is positive deviance in growth simply the converse of negative deviance? Food Nutr Bull 1991;13:7-11.
37. Garmezy N. Resiliency and vulnerability to adverse developmental outcomes associated with poverty. In: Thompson T. Hupp SC, eds. Saving children at risk: poverty and disabilities. Newbury Park, Calif, USA: Sage Publications, 1992:45-60.
38. Rutter M. Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In: Rolf J. Masten AS, Cicchetti D, Nuechterlein KH, Weintraub S. eds. Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990:181-214.
39. Engle PL. Child care and nutrition. Theme paper for the ICN. New York: UNICEF, 1992
40. Black M, Dubowitz H. Failure to thrive: lessons from animal models and developing countries. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1991;2:259-67.
41. Bradley RH, Casey PH, Wortham B. Home environments of low SES non-organic failure-to-thrive infants. Merrill-Palmer Q 1984;30:393-402.
42. Hepner R. Maiden NC. Growth rate, nutrient intake, and "mothering" as determinants of malnutrition in disadvantaged children. Nutr Rev 1971;29:219-23.
43. Zeskind PS, Ramey CT. Fetal malnutrition: an experimental study of its consequences on infant development in two caregiving environments. Child Dev 1979;49:1155-62.
44. Kerr M, Bogues J. Kerr D. Psychosocial functioning of mothers of malnourished children. Pediatrics 1978;62: 77884.
45. Richardson SA. The background histories of school children severely malnourished in infancy. In: Schulman I, ed. Advances in pediatrics Chicago, 111, USA: Yearbook Medical Publications, 1974;21:167-92.
46. Galler JR, Ramsey F. The influence of early malnutrition on subsequent behavioral development: VI. The role of the microenvironment of the household. Nutr Behav 1985;2:16173.
47. Graves P. Nutrition, infant behavior, and maternal characteristics: a pilot study in West Bengal, India. Am J Clin Nutr 1976;29:305-19.
48. Graves PL. Nutrition and infant behavior: a replication study in the Katmandu Valley, Nepal. Am J Clin Nutr 1978;31:54151.
49. Alvarez ML, Wurgaft F. Wilder H. Non verbal language in mothers with malnourished infants. Soc Sci Med 1982;16:1365-9.
50. Valenzuela M. Attachment in chronically underweight young children. Child Dev 1990;61:1984-96.
51. Dixon SD, LeVine RA, Brazelton TB. Malnutrition: a closer look at the problem in an East African village. Dev Med Child Neurol 1982;14:670-85.
52. Goodall J. A social score for kwashiorkor: explaining the look in the child's eyes. Dev Med Child Neurol 1979;21:374-84.
53. Sigman M, Neumann C, Baksh M, Bwibo N. McDonald MA. Relationship between nutrition and development in Kenyan toddlers. J Pediatr 1989;115:357-64.
54. Chavez A, Martinez C, Yachine T. Nutrition, behavioral development, and mother-child interaction in young rural children. Fed Proc 1975;34:1574.
55. Wachs TD, Sigman M, Bishry S. Moussa W. Jerome N. Neumann C, Bwibo N. McDonald MA. Caregiver child interaction patterns in two cultures in relation to nutritional intake. Int J Behav Dev 1992;15:1-18.
56. Engle PL, Zeitlin M. Relations among activity level, anthropometric status, appetite, and feeding frequency in 12-18 month old Nicaraguan children. FASEB J 8(no. 5, part II);1994.
57. Arya S. Infant nutrition and mother-child dyed. Indian J Clin Psych 1989;16:3440.
58. Rahmanifar A, Kirksey A, Wachs T. McCabe GP, Bishry Z. Galal OM, Harrison GG, Jerome NW. Diet during lactation associated with infant behavior and caregiver-infant interaction in a semirural Egyptian village. J Nutr 1993;123:164-75.
59. Levine RA. Omwana: infants and parents in a Kenya community. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1989.
60. Super CM, Herrera MG, Mora JO. Long term effects of food supplementation and psychosocial intervention on the physical growth of Colombian infants at risk of malnutrition. Child Dev 1990;61:29-49.
61. Weisner TS, Gallimore R. My brother's keeper: child and sibling caretaking. Curr Anthropol 1977;18:16990.
62. Engle PL, Breaux C. The father instinct: fathers responsibility for children. Population Council/ICRW Series on Female Headship. New York: Population Council, 1994.
63. Belsky J. The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev 1984;55:83-96.
64. Okagaki L, Divecha DJ. Development of parental beliefs. In: Luster T, Okagaki L, eds. Parenting: an ecological perspective. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: LEA, 1993:3568.
65. Spieker SJ, Bensley L. Roles of living arrangements and grandmother social support in adolescent mothering and infant attachment. Dev Psych 1994;30:102-11.
66. Werner EE, Smith RS. Overcoming the odds: high risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY, USA: Cornell University Press, 1992.
67. Radke-Yarrow M, Brown E. Resilience and vulnerability in children of multiple-risk families. Dev Psychopathol 1993;5:581-92.
68. Engle PL. Discrepancy between Guatemalan women's desires to work and actual employment. In: Sanabary NM, ed. Women and work in the third world. Berkeley, Calif, USA: Center for the Study, Education, and Advancement of Women, 1983:103-16.
69. LeVine RA. Human prenatal care: universal goals, cultural strategies, individual behavior. In: LeVine RA, Miller PM, West MM, eds. Parental behavior in diverse societies. New Directions in Child Development no. 40. San Francisco, Calif, USA: Jossey-Bass, 1988:3-12.
70. Engle PL, La Montagne J. Zeitlin M. Caring behaviors and nutritional status of weaning age children in Managua, Nicaragua. Report to UNICEF. New York: UNICEF, 1992.
71. Bandura A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am Psychol 1982;37:122-47.
72. Engle PL, Zeitlin M, Medrano U. Garcia LH. Growth consequences of low income Nicaraguan mothers' theories of feeding one year olds. In: Super C, Harkness S. eds. Parental ethnotheories. New York: Guilford Press, in press.
73. Detwyler KA. Infant feeding in Mali, West Africa: variations in belief and practice. Soc Sci Med 1986;23: 651-64.
74. Detwyler KA. Breastfeeding and weaning in Mali: cultural context and hard data. Soc Sci Med 1987;24:63344.
75. Bentley ME, Stallings RY, Futumoto M, Elder JA. Maternal feeding behavior and child acceptance of food during diarrhea, convalescence, and health in the central Sierra of Peru. Am J Publ Health 1991;81: 43-7.
76. Brown KH, Dickin KL, Bentley ME, Oni GA, Obassaju VT, Esrey SA, Mebrahtu S. Alade I, Stallings RY. Consumption of weaning foods from fermented cereals: Kwara State, Nigeria. In: Alnwick D, Moses S. Schmidt OG, eds. Improving young child feeding in Eastern and Southern Africa, household level food technology. Proceedings of a workshop held in Nairobi, Kenya, 12 October 1987. Ottawa: IDRC, 1988:181-97.
77. Gibbons G. Griffiths M. Program activities for improving weaning practices. Report prepared for UNICEF. Washington, DC: American Public Health Association, 1984.
78. Engle PL, Davidson K. Effects of maternal self-efficacy on child caregiving and nutritional status. FASEB J 1995;9(no. 5, part II):A5838.
79. Reis J. Child-rearing expectations and developmental knowledge according to maternal age and parity. Infant Mental Health J 1988;9:287-304.
80. Hess RE, Kashiwagi K, Azuma H. Price GG, Dickson WP. Maternal expectations for mastery of developmental tasks in Japan and the United States. Int J Psychol 1980;15:259-71.
81. Luster T. Okagaki L. Parenting: an ecological perspective. Hillside, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1993.
82. Guldan GS, Zeitlin MF, Beiser AS, Super CM, Gershoff SN, Datta S. Maternal education and child feeding practices in rural Bangladesh. Soc Sci Med 1993;36: 925-35.
83. Sartorius N. Depressive illness as a worldwide problem. In: Keilholz P. ed. Depression in everyday practice. Berne, Switzerland: H Huber, 1974.
84. Paltiel L. Women and mental health: a post Nairobi perspective. World Health Stat Q 1987;40:233-66.
85. Chakrabotry S. Social stress and mental health. New Delhi: Sage, 1991.
86. Weisner TS, Abbott S. Women, modernity, and stress: three contrasting contexts for change in East Africa. J Anthropol Res 1977;33:-38.
87. Engle PL. Child care strategies of working and non-working women in rural and urban Guatemala. In: Leslie J. Paolisso M, eds. Women, work, and child welfare in the third world. Boulder, Col, USA: Westview, 1989:179-200.
88. Hewlett BS. Father-child relations, cultural and biosocial contexts. Hawthorne, NY, USA: Aldine de Gruyter, 1992.
89. Werner EE. A cross-cultural perspective on infancy: research and social issues. J Cross-Cultural Psychol 1988;19:96-113.
90. Engle PL. Maternal work and child care strategies in periurban Guatemala: nutritional effects. Child Dev 1991;62:95465.
91. Gottfried AK, Gottfried AW, eds. Maternal employment and children's development: longitudinal research. New York: Plenum Press, 1988.
92. Scarr S. Phillips D, McCartney K. Facts, fantasies, and the future of child care in the United States. Psychol Sci 1990;1:26-35.
93. Carr M, Sandhu R. Women, technology, and rural productivity: an analysis of the impact of time and energy saving technologies on women. UNIFEM Occasional Paper no. 6. New York: UNIFEM, 1987.
94. Acayra M, Bennett L. The rural women of Nepal: an aggregate analysis and summary of eight village studies. Katmandu, Nepal: Center for Economic Development and Administration, Tribhuvan University, 1981.
95. Engle PL. Influences of mothers' and fathers' income on child nutritional status in Guatemala. Soc Sci Med 1993;37:130312.
96. Blumberg RL. Income under female versus male control. J Fam Issues 1988;9:51-84.
97. Dwyer D, Bruce J. A home divided: women and income in the third world. Stanford, Calif, USA: Stanford University Press, 1988.
98. Hoddinott J, Haddad L. Household expenditures, child anthropometric status and the intrahousehold division of income: evidence from the Cote d'Ivoire. Mimeo. Washington, DC: International Food Policy Research Institute, 1991.
99. Doan RM, Bisharat L. Female autonomy and child nutritional status: the extended family residential unit in Amman, Jordan. Soc Sci Med 1990;31:783-90.
100. Begin F, Habicht J-P, Forngily RA, Delisle H. Modification of secular deterioration in child nutritional status by mother's influence on child feeding in rural Chad. FASEB J 1994;8(no. 4, part I):A901.
101. Engle PL, Pederson M. Maternal work for earnings and children's nutritional status in urban Guatemala. Ecol Food Nutr 1989;22:211-23.
102. Vial I, Muchnik E, Mardones F. Women's market work, infant feeding practices, and infant nutrition among low-income women in Santiago, Chile. In: Leslie J, Paolisso M, eds. Women, work, and child welfare in the third world. Boulder, Col, USA: Westview, 1989:131-60.
103. von Braun J. Effects of new export crops in small holder agriculture on division of labor and child nutritional status in Guatemala. In: Leslie J. Paolisso M, eds. Women, work, and child welfare in the third world. Boulder, Col, USA: Westview, 1989:201-16.
104. Hines JR, Landers C, Leslie J. Women, work, and child care. Florence, Italy: Innocenti Centre, UNICEF, 1992.
105. Myers R. The twelve who survive: strengthening programmes of early childhood development in the third world. New York: Routledge, 1992.
106. Borooah R., Cloud K, Seshadri S. Saraswathi TS, Peterson JT, Verma A, eds. Capturing complexity: an interdisciplinary look at women, households and development. Newbury Park, Calif, USA: Sage, 1994.
107. Landers C, Leonard A, eds. Women, work, and the need for child care: a review of UNICEF-supported programmes in Nepal, Ecuador, and Ethiopia. New York: UNICEF, 1992.
108. McKay H. Sinisterra L, McKay A, Gomez H. Loreda P. Improving cognitive ability in chronically deprived children. Science 1978;200:270-8.
109. Bashizi B. Daycare centers in Senegal: a woman's initiative. Assignment Children 1979;47/48:165-71.
110. McBride BA. The effects of a parent education/play group program on father involvement in child rearing. Family Relations 1990;39:250-6.
111. Slaughter D. Early intervention and its effects on maternal and child development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1984, 48, no. 4.
112. Seitz V, Apfel NH. Parent-focused intervention: diffusion effects. Child Dev 1994;65:677-83.
113. George SM, Hass JD, Latham MC. Nutrition education can reduce gender inequality in growth of preschool children in rural south India. FASEB J 1994: 2539A.
114. Aarons A, Hawes H. Child to child. London: Macmillan, 1979.
115. Otaala B. Myers R. Landers C. Children caring for children: new applications of an old idea. New York: UNICEF, 1988.
116. Knight J. Grantham-McGregor S. Using primary school children to improve child-rearing practices in rural Jamaica. Child: Care, Health, and Development 1985;11:81-90.
117. Bruce J. Lloyd CB, Leonard A, Engle PL, Duffy N. Families in focus: new perspectives on mothers, fathers, and children. New York: Population Council, 1995.
118. Levine JA, Murphy DT, Wilson S. Getting men involved: strategies for early childhood programs. New York: Scholastic, 1993.
119. Engle PL, Aletorre J. Taller sobre paternidad responsible. Report of a workshop, Mexico, December 3-6, 1993. New York: Population Council, 1994.
120. McNelly B. Vor der Bruegge E, Watetip C, Eiland M. Credit with education: an innovative strategy developed by Freedom from Hunger for alleviating hunger and malnutrition. FASEB J 1994;8(no. 4, part D: A2541.
121. UNICEF. Iringa: the child survival and development revolution. Joint Nutrition Support Program, New York: UNICEF, 1989.