Contents - Previous - Next

This is the old United Nations University website. Visit the new site at http://unu.edu



Early supplementary feeding, child development, and health policy


Ernesto Pollitt and Se-Young Oh

 


Selection bias


The possibility of bias resulting from a selective review of studies (the "file-drawer problem") was also considered in the analysis. We assumed that the results might have resulted from a failure of investigators to report studies showing a lack of effects, or a failure of journals to publish such findings [20]. The magnitude of such a bias was estimated by calculating the number of unpublished, not statistically significant studies that would be necessary to bring the overall probability value to greater than .05. Twenty-six null-result studies would be necessary to dilute the finding for the motor test at 8-15 months, 2 for the mental test at 18-24 months, and 16 for the motor test at 18-24 months.


Discussion


The results of the meta-analysis agree in part with the hypothesis that drove the analysis. Among nutritionally at-risk populations, protein and calorie supplementation’s during pregnancy and during the first two years of postnatal life provide salutary effects on the motor development of children up to 24 months of age. On the other hand, there is no evidence that early supplementary feeding affects the mental development of children up to the age of 15 months. It does have an effect on mental development among children 18-24 months old.

The scientific merit of the meta-analysis is evident in the assessment of the effects on the mental development scores at 18-24 months. Although only one of the three studies found statistically significant differences between the supplemented and non-supplemented groups, all three found arithmetic differences between the groups in the direction of the proposed hypothesis. This trend became statistically meaningful by pooling the studies, substantiating the inference of a treatment effect on mental development among children 18-24 months old. We therefore conclude that providing high energy and protein supplementary foods to nutritionally at-risk infants and children up to 24 months of age prevents, in part, developmental delays.

The effects derived from prenatal supplementation are likely to result from the transport of nutrients from the mother to the foetus. This concurs with the finding that protein and calorie supplementation during pregnancy among nutritionally at-risk women results in increased birth weight of the offspring [4]. The pathways for the effect of postnatal supplementation are less clear. In reference to the mother, the supplementary food is expected to result in changes in the composition and volume of breast milk. However, data that are available from other studies on this issue show that supplementary feeding to the mother does not affect volume; only suggestive evidence exists that caloric supplementation affects milk composition [21]. An alternative explanation is that nutritional supplements to the mother during pregnancy and lactation have the potential of improving health conditions that will foster her investment in the unborn or born child [4, 22].

The one study that showed no effects on performance on the development scale was carried out in New York [4]. This study, in fact, is also the only one that failed to find an effect on birth weight. Reviewers of the experiment pointed out that the women who received the supplement were not nutritionally at risk, and therefore the absence of benefits on development is not surprising [23]. These findings, when compared with those from the other interventions, suggest that the benefits of early supplementary feeding on development are restricted to populations that are nutritionally at risk.

The benefits observed in performance on the mental development scales in the older group of subjects (15-24 months old) should not be interpreted to mean that the nutrition intervention prevents deficits in later intelligence.

The presence of developmental effects of supplements in the field intervention studies does not guarantee that such effects will be also observed in large-scale food assistance programmes, even if the programmes use supplements and are targeted to populations similar to those of the successful field studies. Tightly controlled experiments are not the same as large-scale programmes, whose effectiveness depends on large bureaucracies and administrative infrastructures that are not very efficient. This is particularly the case in developing countries because of budgetary limitations.

One potential adverse effect of supplementary feeding of young infants is that it can interfere with breastfeeding. Evidence shows that the introduction of other sources of calories results in a reduced intake of breast milk [24]. This substitution is likely to result in a reduction of the immunological benefits from the mother's milk.

It will not be surprising if the findings from this meta-analysis are used to justify food-distribution programmes concerned with young children. However, as we have argued elsewhere [25], evaluation of the merits of these programmes must be placed within the context of national social policies, particularly in developing countries. Food assistance will not compensate for the inefficiency of an educational or a health system, and their existence alone does not define a government's concern for the development of human potential.


Acknowledgements


Data collection and analyses were supported by NIH grant HD22440 and by Pew Charitable Trusts grant 9000210-000.


References


  1. Butler JA, Starfield B. Stenmark S. Child health policy. In: Stevenson HW, Siegel AK, eds. Child development research and social policy. Vol 1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1984:110-88.
  2. Pollitt E, Garza C, Leibel RL. Nutrition and public policy. In: Stevenson HW, Siegel AK, eds. Child development research and social policy Vol 1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1984:421-70.
  3. Graham GG. WIC: a food program that fails (Special Supplemental Food Program for Women, Infants, and Children). Public Interest 1991;103:66-76.
  4. National Academy of Sciences. Nutrition during pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1990.
  5. Rush D, Horwitz DG, Seaver WB et al. Background and introduction. The national WIC evaluation: evaluation of the Special Supplemental Food Program for Women, Infants and Children. Am J Clin Nutr 1988;48(suppl):389-93.
  6. Hicks LE, Langham RA. Cognitive measure stability in siblings following early nutritional supplementation. Public Health Rep 1985;100:656-62.
  7. Hicks LE, Langham RA, Takenaka J. Cognitive and health measures following early nutritional supplementation: a sibling study. Am J Public Health, 1982;72: 1110-8.
  8. Pollitt E, Lorimor R. Effects of WIC on cognitive development. Am J Public Health 1983;73:698-700.
  9. Joos SK, Pollitt E. Effects of supplementation on behavioral development in children up to the age of two years: a comparison of four studies. In: Brozek J. Schürch B. eds. Malnutrition and behavior: critical assessment of key issues. Lausanne, Switzerland: Nestle Foundation 1984: 507-14
  10. Engle PL, Gorman K, Martorell R. Pollin E. Infant and preschool psychological development. Food Nutr Bull 1992;14(3):201-14.
  11. Husaini MA, Karyadi L, Husaini YK, Sandjaja, Karyedi D, Pollitt E. Developmental effects of short-term supplementary feeding in nutritionally at-risk Indonesian infants. Am J Clin Nutr 1991;54:799-804.
  12. Grantham-McGregor SM, Powell CM, Walker SP, Himes JH. Nutritional supplementation, psychosocial stimulation, and mental development of stunted children: the Jamaican study. Lancet 1991;338:1-5.
  13. Waber DP, Vuori-Christiansen L, Ortiz N et al. Nutritional supplementation, maternal education, and cognitive development of infants at risk of malnutrition. Am J Clin Nutr 1981;34:807-13.
  14. Joos SK, Pollitt E, Mueller WH, Albright DL. The Bacon Chow study: maternal nutritional supplementation and infant behavioral development. Child Dev 1983;54:669-76.
  15. Rush D, Stein Z. Susser M. Diet in pregnancy: a randomized controlled trial of nutritional supplements. New York: Alan R. Liss, 1980.
  16. Lasky RE, Klein RE, Yarbrough C, Kallio KD. The predictive validity of infant assessments in rural Guatemala. Child Dev 1981;52:1223-33.
  17. Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Beverly Hills, Calif, USA: Sage, 1984.
  18. Mosteller FM, Bush RR. Selected quantitative techniques. In: Lindsey G. ed. Handbook of social psychology. Vol. 1. Cambridge, Mass, USA: Addison-Wesley, 1954:289-334.
  19. Hedges L. Estimation of effect size from a series of independent experiments. Psychol Bull 1982;92:490-9.
  20. Wolf FM. Meta-analysis: quantitative methods for research synthesis. Quant Appl Soc Sci 1986;59:1-64.
  21. Susser M. Maternal weight gain, infant birth weight, and diet: causal sequences. Am J Clin Nutr 1991;53(6): 138496.
  22. National Academy of Sciences. Nutrition during lactation. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1991.
  23. Haas JD. Nutrition during pregnancy. Science 1981; 212:804-5.
  24. Stuff JE, Nichols BL. Nutrient intake and growth performance of older infants fed human milk. J Pediatr 1989; 115:959-68.
  25. Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle P. Martorell R. Rivera J. Early supplementary feeding and cognition: effect over two decades. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (in press).

Contents - Previous - Next