Contents - Previous - Next

This is the old United Nations University website. Visit the new site at http://unu.edu



Appendix: Description of the tests


Tests for ages 3-7

Embedded figures. The embedded figures test was an assessment of children's ability to distinguish a figure from a meaningful visual array [31]. The construct was developed as a measure of a person's tendency to be field-dependent (influenced by the context) or field-independent. It had two versions.

In the version administered to 3-year-olds the child was shown a picture of a common figure, and then was shown a larger picture that included the same figure and asked to point to the figure. In the first 9 of the 18 items she simply had to select the target figure from a group of other forms; in the second 9 the target figure was embedded in the larger picture. The score was the number of items located correctly.

The version for children 4-7 years old used a red triangle as the target figure. After practice, a copy of the triangle was visible to the child and she was asked to find the triangle hidden in each of 12 pictures.

Verbal inferences. This was a Guatemalan adaptation of a verbal analogies test used in both Stanford-Binet and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC). Two simple test items were first given to the child, and corrections were given if he could not complete an item or completed it incorrectly. A partial sentence was read to the child, who was then expected to complete the idea by supplying the missing word. For example, The first item was: "Shoes go on feet. A hat goes on . . ." The second was: "Water is to drink. Tortillas are to . . ." If the child did not understand the first two examples, the test was not administered.

Vocabulary naming and recognition. The vocabulary test was similar to early items in the Peabody picture vocabulary test. None of the items required inferences about actions. The child was shown a notebook with about four pictures on each page, all of which were of objects common in the village, and was first asked to name each picture. Various synonyms were acceptable. The total number correct was the naming score.

After the child had seen all the pictures, the examiner named each object that the child had not named or had named incorrectly, and the child was asked to point to it. The recognition score was the total number of items named in the first part of the test plus the number recognized in the second part.

Memory for digits. The digit memory test was similar to those in Stanford-Binet and the WISC. The child was asked to repeat sequences of numbers read by the examiner at the rate of two per second. The sequences were of different lengths, the shortest consisting of two digits, with four sequences of each length. The score was the total number of digits recalled correctly plus one point for each totally correct sequence.

Memory for sentences. The child was asked to repeat meaningful sentences after the examiner, who read them at the rate of two words per second. All the words were two syllables long—for example, Mama habla. The sentences had different numbers of words, with two sentences of each length. The score was the total number of words recalled correctly plus one point for each correct sentence.

Draw a line slowly. This test was developed to measure impulse control [30]. It was significantly associated with Stanford-Binet scores in a preschool population but was unrelated to the overall activity level measured by "actometers." It may measure ability to follow instructions as well as impulse control.

The child was asked to draw a line between two X marks on a page as quickly as possible, and then to draw one as slowly as possible. The score was derived from the latter: it was the velocity of the line, i.e., the time to draw the line divided by its length. A lower score indicates greater impulse control.

Persistence on impossible puzzle. The child was given 18 pieces to fit into a puzzle, in an attempt to measure persistence in an impossible situation. In reality there was no way in which all of the pieces could be fitted into the puzzle. A maximum of three minutes was allowed, and the score was the number of 10-second intervals the child continued to work.

Reversal discrimination learning. Children were tested on their ability to make a reversal shift, that is, to transfer learning from one situation to another with reversed requirements.

Four sets of trials to discriminate between two objects were given. In the first set, the child was trained to make a distinction between the objects based on their size. If she selected the correct member of the pair (e.g., the large house), she found a piece of candy under the stimulus. The second set of trials involved discrimination on the same dimension but with different objects (e.g., cars, with the large one still being correct). If the child understands the notion of dimension, she should master the second exercise much more quickly than the first. In the third set of trials, a reversal was introduced: the same two items were presented as in the first trials (a large and a small house), but this time the small house was the correct one. The test of the child's learning from the reversal shift came in the fourth set, when she was shown the same two objects as in the second set (cars), but again with the opposite size (small) now being correct. If, in the first or the third set, the child failed five times in a row, the test was suspended. The score was the total number of trials the child took to reach the criterion.

Memory for objects. As a standard memory test and a measure of ability to use categories of objects to enhance recall, the child was shown a large circular tablet with 12 familiar objects—belonging to three conceptual categories (animals, clothing, kitchen utensils)—placed around the edge. After he had inspected the objects, they were covered with a cloth and he was asked to recall as many as possible. The score was the number of objects recalled.

Knox cubes. In this adaptation of the Arthur Point Scale of Intelligence [61], the child was asked to repeat a series of taps on four stimuli. A board with four familiar miniature objects lined up in a row (a fan, a bow, etc.) was shown to the child. The examiner tapped these items in a particular order with a stick the length of a pencil, and the child was then asked to tap them in the same order. The score was the number of series of taps correctly completed.

In the slow version, the examiner tapped the objects at the rate of one per second. The fast version, which was not used here, has four taps per second and follows the slow version.

 

Tests for ages 5-7

Memory for designs. This test was developed to assess children's ability to remember and reproduce a design made with one-inch cubic blocks painted a different colour on each side (red, green, yellow, and blue, plus some with diagonals). The examiner constructed a design from the blocks (using two blocks for the first two items and four blocks thereafter) and allowed the child to inspect it for five seconds. The blocks were then scrambled and the child was asked to reconstruct the design from memory. Three trials were allowed per design. The score was a number of points based on the correct colour and position of the blocks.

Incidental and intentional learning. This test measured children's ability to recall information they had not been directed to remember. It consisted of two trials.

In the first trial, the child was shown six pictures of common objects, one at a time, and asked to point to the part that was red. After finishing, she was asked to remember what the pictures were. The number of pictures correctly recalled was her incidental learning score.

She was then shown the six pictures again and asked to name them. Then she was asked to name the pictures again from memory. The number correctly recalled on the second trial was her intentional learning score.

Haptic-visual matching. This test was developed in response to the hypothesis that malnourished children are deficient in their ability to integrate among their senses (intersensory integration).

The child was asked to place his hands into a box with a little curtain over the opening and feel an object inside the box (a wooden figure about one and a half centimetres thick, like a jigsaw puzzle piece, glued onto a board) without being able to see it. He was then shown four similar forms and asked to pick the one that was the same shape as the one he had felt. There were 12 objects to be felt in all. The number of correct responses and the response times were recorded.

Matching familiar figures. This test was designed to measure a child's cognitive style: "reflectivity" (long response times and correct answers) or "impulsivity" (short response times and incorrect answers).

The child was shown a drawing of a familiar object and asked to select the one that matched it from a display of similar figures differing only in small details from the stimulus item. The display was covered with a sheet of paper until time for her to start searching so that her response time could be measured accurately. Because of a tendency in such tests for children to respond immediately to the figure in the lower right-hand corner, closest to their hand, the location of the correct response and the arrangement of the alternatives on the page were varied. Both the number of items selected correctly and the response times were recorded.

Block design. The child was shown a design on a card and asked to replicate it using a set of small, coloured cubical blocks. Some of the more difficult designs involved blocks with diagonal lines. The score was based on points for the correct colour, position, and orientation for each block plus one point for each completely correct design.

Animal houses. This test of non-verbal intelligence from the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPSSI) measures the child's ability to develop an arbitrary code pairing two stimuli. The child was first presented with four animals and shown that each one had a "house" of a different colour. He was then given a tablet with 20 pictures of animals— 5 copies of each of the four standards—and a set of coloured pegs, and was asked to place a peg of the colour of the animal's "house" under each picture as quickly as possible within a time limit. Both the time taken to complete the task and the number of correct responses were recorded.

Elimination of odd figure. The child was shown a series of five drawings in a row and asked to point to the one that was different. In some cases the odd figure differed on perceptual grounds (e.g., a child wearing black rather than white shoes) and in some on conceptual grounds (e.g., one child in the picture was engaged in a different activity from the others). The score was the total number of items correctly identified.

Face-hands touching. To test children's ability to determine on which side of their body a touch occurs, they were touched with a feather on both the face and the hands with their eyes closed and were asked to say whether the touches were on the same or different sides. The score was the number of touches correctly identified.

Incomplete figures. Both Stanford-Binet and the WPSSI contain some measure of children's ability to identify the part of a picture of a common object that is missing. The child was shown pictures of 16 items common to Guatemalan village life and was asked to point out the missing part. The score was the total number of missing parts correctly identified.

Conservation of matter. This adaptation of Piaget's conservation-of-mass test used the shapes of tortillas and salchichas (hot dogs) as stimuli in order to increase its relevance. The child was shown two balls of clay and was asked whether there was the same amount in each. The examiner then moulded the balls into different forms and afterwards reshaped the clay into a ball. After each transformation the child was asked to judge whether the quantity of clay was the same as in the initial comparison. The results were scored on a three-point scale: 0 if the child never recognized that the quantity was conserved, 1 if she recognized it in some but not all of her responses, and 2 if she recognized it every time.

Conservation of area. In this culturally appropriate adaptation of Piaget's conservation-of-area test the child was shown two large round green tablets representing fields, with a toy cow on each, and small blocks representing houses. Both "fields" were first presented in the same configuration to test for the child's recognition of their initial equality. Then, for the transformation, on one tablet the houses were grouped together and on the other they were scattered, and the child was asked whether the two cows still had the same amount to eat. The scores were 0, 1, or 2 as above.

Conservation of continuous quantity. This adaptation of Piaget's conservation-of-liquid test involved three transformations of water in glasses, using different sizes and numbers of glasses. The child was asked if the quantities of water were equal initially, then was shown a transformation and again asked if the quantities were the same. The scoring was the same as for the two conservation tests described above.


Acknowledgements


Data collection and analyses were supported by NIH grant HD22440. The study was a collaborative effort involving investigators at several institutions: R. Martorell (principal investigator, originally at Stanford University, now at Cornell University), J. Rivera (INCAP, Guatemala), E. Pollitt (University of California at Davis), and J. Haas (Cornell University).


References


  1. Pollin E. Gorman K, Engle PL, Martorell R. Rivera J. Early supplementary feeding and cognition: effects over two decades. Monagr Soc Res Child Dev (in press).
  2. Waterlow JC, Cravioto J. Stephen JKL. Protein malnutrition in man. Advances in protein chemistry. New York: Academic Press, 1960: 15-238.
  3. Cravioto J. Malnutrition and behavioral development in the preschool child. In: Preschool child malnutrition. Washington, DC: National Research Council, 1966:7484.
  4. Cravioto J. DeLicardie RE, Birch HG. Nutrition, growth, and neuro-integrative development: an experimental and ecologic study. Pediatrics 1966; 38(suppl 2):319-72.
  5. Monckenberg F. Effect of early marasmic malnutrition on subsequent physical and psychological development. In: Scrimshaw NS, Gordon JE, eds. Malnutrition, learning and behavior. Cambridge, Mass, USA: MIT Press, 1968:269-79.
  6. Stock Ms. Smythe PM. Undernutrition during infancy and subsequent brain growth and intellectual development. In: Scrimshaw NS, Gordon JE, eds. Malnutrition, learning and behavior. Cambridge, Mass, USA: MIT Press, 1968:278-88.
  7. Engsner G. Vahlquist B. Brain growth in children with protein-energy malnutrition. In: Brazier MAB, ed. Growth and development of the brain: nutritional, genetic, and environmental factors. New York: Raven Press, 1975: 315-34.
  8. Coursin DB. Malnutrition, brain development and behavior: anatomic, biochemical, and electrophysiologic constructs. In Brazier MAB, ed. Growth and development of the brain: nutritional, genetic, and environmental factors. New York: Raven Press, 1975:289-305.
  9. Von Muralt A. Influence of early protein-calorie malnutrition on the intellectual development: the point of view of a physiologist. In: Brazier MAB, ed. Growth and development of the brain: nutritional, genetic, and environmental factors. New York: Raven Press, 1975:307-14.
  10. Davison AN, Dobbing J. The developing brain. In: Davison AN, Dobbing J. eds. Applied neurochemistry. Colchester, UK: William Clowes and Sons, 1968:25386.
  11. Dobbing J. Vulnerable periods in developing brain. In: Davison AN, Dobbing J. eds. Applied neurochemistry. Coichester, UK: William Clowes and Sons, 1968:287316.
  12. Knobloch H. Pasamanik B. Predicting intellectual potential in infancy. Am J Dis Child 1963; 106:43-51.
  13. Cravioto J. DeLicardie RE. Intersensory development of school aged children. In: Scrimshaw NS, Gordon JE, eds. Malnutrition, learning and behavior. Cambridge, Mass, USA: MIT Press, 1968:252-69.
  14. Birch HG, Belmont L. Auditory-visual integration, intelligence, and reading ability in school children. Percep Motor Skills 1965;20:295-305.
  15. Richardson SA. Severity of malnutrition in infancy and its relation to later intelligence. In: Brozek J. ed. Behavioral effects of energy and protein deficits. (NIH publication no. 79-1906) Washington, DC: Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1979:172-84.
  16. Scrimshaw NS, Gordon JE. Malnutrition, learning, and behavior. Cambridge, Mass, USA: MIT Press, 1968.
  17. Read MS. Behavioral correlates of malnutrition. In: Brazier MAB, ed. Growth and development of the brain: nutritional, genetic, and environmental factors. New York: Raven Press, 1975:335-54.
  18. Frankova S. Influence of nutrition and early experience on behavior in rats. Bibl Nutr Diet 1972;17:96-110.
  19. Frankova S. Interaction between early malnutrition and stimulation in animals. In: Cravioto J. Hamberays L, Vahiquist B. eds. Early malnutrition and mental development. Symposium of the Swedish Nutrition Foundation, XII, Uppsala. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell, 1974:202-09.
  20. Levitsky DA, Barnes RH. Malnutrition and the biology of experience. In: Chavez A, Bourges H. Basta S. eds. Proceedings of the IXth International Congress of Nutrition, Mexico City. Vol 2. Basel: S. Larger, 1975:33034.
  21. Thomson AM. Historical perspectives of nutrition, reproduction and growth. In: Scrimshaw NS, Gordon JE, eds. Malnutrition, learning, and behavior. Cambridge, Mass, U SA: MIT Press, 1968: 17- 28.
  22. McLaren DS. A fresh look at protein-energy malnutrition. Lancet 1966;2:485.
  23. Viteri FE, Torun B. Nutrition, physical activity and growth. In: Ritzen M, Aperia A, Hall K, Larsson A, Zetterverg A, Zetterstrom R. eds. The biology of normal growth. New York: Raven Press, 1981:265-73.
  24. Beaton G. Energy in human nutrition: perspectives and problems. Nutr Rev 1983;41:325-430.
  25. Martorell R. Klein RE, Delgado H. Improved nutrition and its effects on anthropometric indicators of nutritional status. Nutr Rep Int 1980;21 :219-30.
  26. Engle PL, Carmichael SL, Gorman K, Pollitt E. Demographic and socio-economic changes in families in four Guatemalan villages, 1967-1987. Food Nutr Bull 1992;14(3):237-45.
  27. Bayley N. Manual for the Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1969.
  28. Lasky RE, Klein RE, Yarbrough C, Engle PL, Lechtig A, Martorell R. The relationship between physical growth and infant behavioral development in rural Guatemala. Child Dev 1981;52:219-26.
  29. Lasky RE, Klein RE, Yarbrough C, Kallio KD. The predictive validity of infant assessments in rural Guatemala. Child Dev 1981;53:847-56.
  30. Maccoby EE, Dowley EM, Hagen JW, Degerman R. Activity level and intellectual functioning in normal preschool children. Child Dev 1965;36:761-70.
  31. Witkin H. Psychological differentiation: studies of development. New York: Wiley, 1962.
  32. Kendier TS, Kendier HH. Reversed and non-reversed shift in kindergarten children. J Exp Psych 1959;58:5660.
  33. White SH. Some general outlines of the matrix of developmental changes between five and seven years. Bull Orton Soc 1970;20:41-57.
  34. Klein RE, Freeman HE, Spring B. Nerlove SR, Yarbrough C. Cognitive test performance and indigenous conceptions of intelligence. J Psych 1976;93:273-79.
  35. Nerlave SB, Roberts JM, Klein RE, Yarbrough C, Habicht JP. Natural indicators of cognitive development: an observational study of rural Guatemalan children. Ethos 1974;2:265-95.
  36. Irwin M, Engle PL, Yarbrough C, Klein RE, Townsend J. The relationship of prior ability and family characteristics to school attendance and school achievement in rural Guatemala. Child Dev 1978; 49:415-27.
  37. Klein RE, Arenales P. Delgado H et al. Effects of maternal nutrition on fetal growth and infant development. Bull Pan Am Health Org 1976;10:301-06.
  38. Townsend JW, Klein RE, Irwin MH, Owens W. Yarbrough C, Engle PL. Nutrition and preschool mental development. In: Wagner DA, Stevenson HW, eds. Cross-cultural perspectives on child development. San Francisco, Calif, USA: WH Freeman, 1982:124-45.
  39. Freeman H. Klein RE, Townsend JW, Lechtig A. Nutrition and cognitive development among rural Guatemalan children. Am J Pub Health 1980;70:127785.
  40. Engle PL, Irwin M, Klein PE, Yarbrough C, Townsend JW. Nutritional and mental development in children. In: Winick M, ed. Nutrition: pre and postnatal influences. New York: Plenum Press, 1979:291-306.
  41. Engle PL, Yarbrough C, Klein RE. Sex differences in the effects of nutrition and social environment on mental development in rural Guatemala. In: Buvinic M, Lycette MA, McGreevoy WP, eds. Women's issues in third world poverty. Baltimore, Md, USA: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1983:198-215.
  42. Barrett DE, Radke-Yarrow M, Klein RE. Chronic malnutrition and child behavior: effects of early caloric supplementation on social and emotional functioning at school age. Dev Psych 1982;18(4):541-56.
  43. Barrett DE, Radke-Yarrow M. Effects of nutritional supplementation on children's responses to novel, frustrating and competitive situations. Am J Clin Nutr 1985 ;42: 102-20.
  44. Cook TO, Campbell DR. Quasi-experimentation: design and analysis for field settings. Chicago, Ill, USA: Rand McNally, 1979.
  45. Engle PL, Levin R. Sex differences in the effects of malnutrition on mental development: a review and some hypotheses. In: Schurch B. Brozek J. eds. Malnutrition and behavior: a critical assessment of key issues. Lausanne, Switzerland: Nestlé, 1984:396-410.
  46. Super CM, Herrera MG, Mora JO. Long-term effects of food supplementation and psychosocial intervention on the physical growth of Colombian infants at risk of mainutrition. Child Dev 1990;61:29-49.
  47. Adair LS, Pollitt E. Outcome of maternal nutritional supplementation: a comprehensive review of the Bacon Chow study. Am I Clin Nutr 1985;41:948-78.
  48. Super CM, Herrera MG, Mora JO. Cognitive outcomes of early nutrition intervention in the Bogota study. Abstracts of the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle. Chicago, Ill, USA: Society for Research in Child Development, 1991 ;61 :29-491.
  49. Hsueh AM, Meyer B. Maternal dietary supplementation and five-year-old Stanford-Binet IQ on the offspring in Taiwan. Fed Proc 1981;40:897.
  50. Joos SK, Pollitt E, Mueller WH, Albright DA. The Bacon Chow study: maternal nutritional supplementation and infant behavioral development. Child Dev 1983;54:669-76.
  51. Chavez A, Martinez C. Growing up in a developing community. Guatemala: INCAP, 1982.
  52. Malina HT. Technical report of general ability and tests of reading. Interamerican Series. San Antonio, Tex, USA: Guidance Testing Associates, 1984.
  53. Rutishauser HE, Whitchead RG. Energy intake and expenditure in 1-3 year old Ugandan children living in a rural environment. Br J Nutr 1972;28:145-52.
  54. Engle PL, Zeitlin M, Medrano Y. Parental thtories about feeding behaviors for undernourished infants. Child Dev Abstr Bibl 1991;8:67.
  55. Grantham-McGregor S. Meeks Gardner JM, Walker S. Powell C. The relationship between undernutrition, activity levels and development in young children. In: Schurch B. Scrimshaw NSS, eds. Proceedings of the meeting of the International Dietary Energy Consultative Group. Lausanne, Switzerland: Nestlé, 1990:36183.
  56. Campos JJ, Bertenthal Bl. Locomotion and psychological development in infancy. In: Morrison FM, Lord C, Keating D, eds. Applied developmental psychology. Vol 3. New York: Academic Press, 1990:229-58.
  57. Gustafson GE. Effects of the ability to locomote on infants' social and exploratory behaviors: an experimental study. Dev Psych 1984;20:397-405.
  58. Huttenlocher J. Haight W. Burk A, Seltzer M, Lyons T. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev Psych 1991 ;27(2):236-48.
  59. Sigman M, Neumann C, Carter E, Cattle DJ. Home interactions and the development of Embu toddiers in Kenya. Child Dev 1988;59:1251-61.
  60. Munroe RH, Munroe RL. Infant experiences and childhood cognition: a longitudinal study among the Logoli of Kenya. Ethos 1984;13:291-306.
  61. Leiter RG. The Leiter international performance scale. Santa Barbara, Calif, USA: Santa Barbara State College Press, 1940.

Contents - Previous - Next